Navigation – Plan du site

AccueilNuméros61Numeral systems in Mande languages

Numeral systems in Mande languages1

Systèmes des numéraux dans les langues mandé
Системы числительных в языках манде
Elena Perekhvalskaya et Valentin Vydrin

Résumés

L’article présente une analyse détaillée de huit systèmes de compte mandé (bambara traditionnel et bambara moderne, boko, dzuungoo, mwan, dan-gwɛɛtaa, san-maka, soninké), ensuite un tableau synthétique des particularités des systèmes numériques de 30 langues représentant tous les groupes de la famille mandé est présenté. Dans les langues de la famille mandé, on trouve des éléments de différents systèmes numériques : décimal, vigésimal, octogésimal, et quelques éléments des systèmes quinaires et quindécimaux (à base 15) sont également représentés. Dans quelques langues, des numéraux complexes peuvent être formés par soustraction, le nombre soustrait pouvant aller jusqu’à 25. Dans le système traditionnel bambara, jogo-ton et jogo-ligbi, le procédé de division est utilisé, ce qui est extrêmement rare du point de vue typologique. Dans le compte de la monnaie, des nombreuses langues mandé utilisent le procédé de multiplication par cinq.

En guise de conclusion, il est proposé que les systèmes quinaires et décimaux sont originels dans les langues mandé, tandis que les systèmes vigésimaux (de même que quindécimaux et octogésimaux) proviennent d’une évolution récente, probablement sous l’influence (contactes ou substrat) des langues sénoufo, gur, et probablement aussi kru.

Haut de page

Texte intégral

  • 1 This work is related to research strand 3 “Typology and dynamics of linguistic systems” of the Lab (...)

1Suite à des difficultés techniques indépendantes de notre volonté, les diacritiques tonales peuvent apparaître avec un léger décalage en fonction du navigateur internet utilisé. Pour un affichage optimal, nous vous recommandons de consulter cette page avec le navigateur Firefox ou bien de télécharger gratuitement le fichier PDF.

1. Introduction

2The counting systems and systems of numerals found in Mande languages are rather heterogeneous, and some of these systems display unique or, at least, typologically rare features. In this paper, we analyse these systems from a typological viewpoint, taking into account ideas formulated in fundamental works which have treated numerals in the languages of the world from universalist, theoretical and typological points of view (Greenberg 1978; Hurford 1975). We begin our analysis by presenting a conceptual framework and terminology for the study of counting systems. In the main part, we investigate eight numeral systems (those of old Bamana, modern Bamana, Boko, Dzuungoo, Mwan, Dan-Gwɛɛtaa, San-Maka, and Soninke) that represent different types of identifiable systems. Next, the data for these and some other Mande languages will be represented in a table, followed by comments on the most interesting features of individual languages. The paper ends with an analysis of general trends and particularities of numeral systems in the Mande language family.

3The main goal of this paper is to represent the Mande language data in a more or less standard format, so that these data might be easily comparable, and to provide a primary analysis. This paper is therefore data-oriented rather than theory-oriented.

4The data represented in this paper are partly drawn from the field materials of the authors (especially for Bamana, Dan-Gwɛɛtaa, Mwan, San-Maka); otherwise, the data are provided with references for their sources.

1.1. Restricted systems and body part counting.

5Typologically, all numeral systems in the languages of the world are either restricted with no arithmetic base or non-restricted.

  • 2 Cf. (Comrie 2005): “some languages have restricted numeral systems, by which I mean more specifica (...)

6Restricted number systems consist of several elements, usually not more than five, and often of three or four numerals.2 Numerals in these systems denote sets of concrete objects, rather than abstract numbers.

7We assume that the emergence of numeral systems which have an arithmetic base is a relatively recent phenomenon. According to the World Atlas of Language Structures (Comrie 2005), there still exist “restricted” systems, which use three or four numerals. Larger numbers here are expressed by combinations of these few elements. Within the framework of such a system, the expression of numbers larger than 20 is extremely cumbersome, and as a result, there are limits of the application of exact counting (traces of words with meanings like “the final number” are attested in many languages whose systems are no longer restricted). Such systems are typical of small closed communities, which, until recently, were unconcerned by commercial relations, had no monetary system and conducted a traditional way of life based on hunting and gathering.

8As a rule, these restricted systems coexist with extended body-part counting. In the course of such counting, parts of the body are named in an established order, and the name of a body part is used as the designation of a certain set (“fist” > five). Primarily, fingers and finger phalanges are used for counting.

  • 3 For concrete examples of such systems see (Craig 2010; Saxe & Esmonde 2004).

9This technique allows, if necessary, for the accurate naming of numbers up to a hundred and even beyond.3 For larger numbers calculations use the body parts of a second and then a third person, and so on. It would be logical to assume that this practice explains the formation of more widespread positional (cyclic) systems of numerals. Most likely they originated from a system of counting by body parts.

10Certain terms for numbers in Mande languages can be traced back to body parts: hand, foot, mouth, head, human being, a fact that can be explained in terms of original body-part counting. At the same time, there are terms for higher numerals which go back to the names of certain sets: string of cowries, basket of cola nuts.

11Systems with an arithmetic base (all modern Mande systems belong to this type) are arranged similarly. The structure of a compound numeral in these systems can be described by Bernard Comrie's formula (Comrie n.d.): for a b-based numeral system, (n x b)+m (where m < b). Arithmetic operations are either implied (with the components combined through parataxis) or marked by auxiliary words (connectors).

1.2. Base of the counting system

12Following (Comrie 1997; Comrie 1999), “by the “base” of a numeral system we mean the value n such that numeral expressions are constructed according to the pattern ... xn + y, i.e. some numeral x multiplied by the base plus some other numeral”.

  • 4 See (Greenberg 1990: 281) on the importance of distinguishing between the augend and addend, the m (...)

13It seems necessary to distinguish between the bases, which are used in multiplication (i.e., multiplicands), and the numerals used as the first component in addition (i.e., augends);4 in systems where subtraction is used, we can also single out the first components used in subtraction (minuends). By default, multiplicands (sole or together with their multipliers) can also be used in numerative systems as augends; in some languages, they can also serve as minuends.

14The multiplicand of the lower order, i.e., the radix (Greenberg's “fundamental base”), defines the type of the numerical system. Different bases can be combined within a single numerative system, rudiments of older systems being preserved. The following radixes are registered in natural languages (this list is certainly not exhaustive):

Radix Type of the system
4 quaternary
5 quinary
10 decimal
12a duodecimal
15 pentadecimal (or quindecimal)
20 vigesimal
60 sexagesimal
80 octodecimal
a. “The question is how could a twelve-digit system ever appear? Examples of this count: the division of the year in 12 months, the count of time (12 hours a day), the division of the hour in 60 minutes, and 3600 seconds, the division of the circle in 360 degrees. This system was accepted in Europe, and then spread all over the world. Its origin is usually associated with Babylon and is determined by observations of the movement of the Moon <...> But how the achievements of the Ancient East could reach the remote mountains in the depths of Africa is unclear <...> Therefore, one should search the origin of this system in the hand count, and not fingers, but 12 phalanges” (Olderogge 1982: 28).

15In most instances, base (multiplicands), as well as augends and minuends, are simple (non-derived and non-compound) forms, although some exceptions to this rule can be found. For instance, in Boko there are augends ‘15’ and ‘25’ which are compound words.

16Dmitry Olderogge singled out several counting systems in Africa which are differentiated by the number of “counting thresholds” (i. e., lower multiplicands and augends) they display:

  • A four-stage system: thresholds 5, 10, 15, 20. Numbers from 1 through 5 are simple; numbers from 6 through 9 follow the quinary model; numbers from 11 through 14 are derived from 10, and those from 16 through 19 are based on 15. The subsequent numerals are based on 20.
  • A three-stage system: thresholds 5, 10, 20.
  • A two-stage system: the thresholds are 10 and 20; no traces of the quinary model.
  • Complex vigesimal systems “existing among the communities who used to count cowries”. The counting is decimal or/and vigesimal; however, there are special words for 400, 600, etc. (Olderogge 1984: 3–4).

1.3. Arithmetic in counting systems.

17Bernard Comrie (2005) mentions two operations of arithmetic implicitly used in counting systems: addition and multiplication. Quite a few systems also use subtraction, e.g. Latin duo de viginti ‘eighteen’ (2 from 20).‘’

18Division in the numeral systems of natural languages is extremely rare. So, in (Comrie 1997) only Welsh is mentioned in this relation, where hanner cant ‘50’ is ‘half hundred’. According to Greenberg (1990: 280), if the numeral system of a language applies division, «‘half’ is the “unmarked fraction” and is almost always a simple lexical item, often derived from ‘to split’, or 'to break' or the like» (Greenberg adduces Orya (Indo-Aryan) as an exception, where ‘quarter’ is also used in the formation of compound numerals).

1.4. Classifiers or counting words.

19Some languages use classifiers, more or less grammaticalized elements located between the numeral and the noun designating the object counted, along the lines of loaves in two loaves of bread (rather than two breads — although English loaf is not a true classifier word but a noun, in view of the lack of grammaticalization). The term “classifiers” is usually applied to languages of the Far East and Southeastern Asia and to some Amerindian languages, where the elements in question have reached an advanced stage of grammaticalization. Where the level of grammaticalization is more moderate, the terms “counting words” and “measure words” are usually preferred; this is the case in certain Mande languages. We provide information about such words whenever it is available. It is reasonable to think that they are in fact much more widespread, but often passed over in silence by the authors of grammatical descriptions.

1.5. Specialized counting systems.

20Two (or more) different systems may coexist simultaneously in a language, so that the same number can be coded in more than one way (twenty-four and two dozen). Typically, the subsidiary system is used for counting special objects (eggs, notches, stitches in knitting, tube diameter and the like).

  • 5 An anonymous reviewer commented that in these contexts, “it is not obvious that the numeral denote (...)

21In West Africa, money-counting often has features of a specialized system. In most Mande languages (and not only in Mande, cf. (Grandet 1973; Zepp 1983)) spoken in the Francophone countries, when money is counted, each numeral (with the exception of higher ones such as millions and billions) designates an amount five times higher than with other objects. So, in modern Bamana mɔ̀bili kɛ̀mɛ fìla means ‘200 cars’, but dɔ́rɔmɛ kɛ̀mɛ fìla is ‘1000 CFA francs’ (lit. ‘200 coins’); in Boko da̋á dó is ‘5 francs’ (lit. ‘one coin’); in Mwan māā kɛ̀mɛ̀ dō is ‘100 hens’, but gɔ̀lí kɛ̀mɛ̀ dō is ‘500 francs’ (lit. ‘100 coins’); wáá yīzīɛ̄ ‘20,000’ (lit. 4 000), in Dzuungoo dāsī fíí ‘10 francs’ (lit. ‘two coins’).5

  • 6 Gourde is the name of the national currency of Haiti since 1813. The word seems to go back to the (...)
  • 7 In modern Guinean Maninka, the word dàlasi means ‘one Guinean franc’. This semantic shift (”5 fra (...)

22This rule of multiplication by five has a historical explanation. In the eighteenth century and in the first half of the nineteenth century, the most prevalent coin in circulation in West Africa was the Spanish silver dollar, which made dollar the default name for a coin. At the same period, a French coin, the gourde, equal to 5 francs, was also in circulation with a value more or less equal to the Spanish dollar (Curtin 1975: 264–270). Presumably in West African languages the term for the dollar was extended to the gourde (5 francs),6 and when (with French colonization) the French franc became the main and, eventually, the only currency, the word which originally meant ‘dollar’ (Bamana dálasi, Dzuungoo dāsī, etc.) was retained for the 5 fr. coin and remained the basic unit of money counting.7 Incidentally, in Gambia the word Dalasi has been retained as the name of the national currency.

23Less clear is the Bamana word for ‘5 francs’, dɔ́rɔ̌mɛ́ (which is much more frequently used than its synonym dálasi). It is generally believed (cf., for example, (Lagarde 1988)) that dɔ́rɔ̌mɛ́ goes back to ‘dirham’, a currency used in Arabic countries (whose name in turn goes back to the Greek drachma). However, the dirham was always a coin of relatively small denomination; its weight was usually close to 3 g of silver. The Spanish dollar in the 18th and 19th centuries ranged between 24 and 30 g of silver. Therefore the dirham could not have been its monetary equivalent. One plausible account then is that the word dɔ́rɔ̌mɛ́, at some point, lost its connection with the dirham and became a default name for a coin, thus becoming a synonym of dálasi.

24In due course this system was sometimes extended, at least in Guro and Mwan (but not in Bamana or Jula), to other currencies. In these languages, not only CFA francs are counted using the “multiply by five” rule, but also dollars, euros, etc.

25In old Bamana, special terms were used when counting cowrie shells (for more detail see the end of section 2.1).

1.6. Internal and external syntax of numerals.

26By “internal syntax of numerals” we mean the manner in which the elements of compound numerals are combined, and by “external syntax”, we mean the way numerals interact with the nouns they determine.

27Unfortunately, we do not have sufficient data on the external syntax of numerals in every language dealt with in this paper. However, we think that there is still some value in providing information on just a limited sub-sample of languages (especially those displaying some unusual features with respect to the common Mande background).

1.7. Etymologies

28Reconstruction of numeral proto-forms is not among the primary tasks of the present paper. However, some potential etymologies of particular forms will be proposed, whenever they are appropriate and useful for understanding the organization of a numeral system. Quite recently, a reconstruction of Mande numerals, against the background of the other Niger-Congo languages, was the subject of a special study by Konstantin Pozdniakov (2018). It seems appropriate to present here Pozdniakov's Proto-Mande etymologies to serve as a point of reference for our “local” etymologies. In some cases, we use brackets to add our own suggestions to the reconstructions (a dot under a letter indicates that the vowel belongs to the –ATR series).

29‘1’ Two roots are candidates for the Proto-Mande level, *do (*dọ́ ) and *kelen, of these *do seems to be better supported.

30‘2’ *pila or *fila (*pìɗà).

  • ‘3’ *sakpa/*sagba/*sawa.
  • ‘4’ The Proto-Mande form*náání/ nɑ̃ɑ̃i is preserved in the Proto-Western Mande, while *sḭ̀ḭ̀yá̰ is a Proto-Southeastern Mande innovation.
  • ‘5’ *dúuru/*sɔ́ɔ́ru (*ɗọ́ọɗụ́ ).
  • ‘6’ = ‘5+1’.
  • ‘7’ = ‘5+2’.
  • ‘8’ unclear (probably ‘5+3’, although other etymologies may also be possible).
  • ‘9’ = ‘5+4’ or ‘10-1’.
  • ‘10’ *pu/*fu (*pú ); *tan is a Proto-Western Mande innovation stemming from the Niger-Congo root for ‘5’.
  • ‘20’ < ‘human being’.
  • ‘100’ *kɛmɛ (may be a recent itinerant word) or ‘20x5’.
  • ‘1000’ *wulu, *waga (may be recent itinerant words).

1.8. Scope of the study

31We have left certain topics outside our study: in particular, the range of problems concerning ordinal numerals, distributive numerals, and the adverbial use of numerals. We have also left aside some idiosyncratic features of individual numeral systems which have no bearing on our generalizations.

1.9. Mande family

32For the convenience of readers, we reproduce here the genealogical tree of the Mande family, in a form of a hierarchical list (the inner classification follows (Vydrin 2009), in a simplified way). The language names are supplemented by ISO 639-3 codes (in italics). Names of those languages whose numeral systems are analyzed in detail are given in bold; the names of the other languages represented in section 2 are underlined. If a language is known under different names, the alternative names are given in brackets. For some groups containing numerous closely related varieties with a status intermediate between a language and a dialect (such as the Manding group), the lists of varieties given here may not be exhaustive.

  • 1. Western Mande
  • 1.1. Manding
  • 1.1.1. Bamana (Bambara) bam
  • 1.1.2. Jula dyu
  • 1.1.3. Maninka man
  • 1.1.4. Mandinka mnk
  • 1.1.5. Xasonka kao
  • 1.2. Mokole
  • 1.2.1. Mogofin (Mikhifore) mfg
  • 1.2.2. Kakabe kke
  • 1.2.3. Koranko knk
  • 1.2.4. Lele llc
  • 1.3. Vai-Kono
  • 1.3.1. Vai vai
  • 1.3.2. Kono kno
  • 1.4. Jogo-Jeri
  • 1.4.1. Jogo
  • 1.4.1.1. Jogo-Ton
  • 1.4.1.2. Jogo-Ligbi lig
  • 1.4.2. Jeri jek
  • 1.4.3. Jalkunan (Ble) bxl
  • 1.5. Southwestern–Susu
  • 1.5.1. Susu–Yalunka
  • 1.5.1.1. Susu (Soso) sus
  • 1.5.1.2. Yalunka (Jalonke) yal
  • 1.5.2. Southwestern Mande
  • 1.5.2.1. Kpelle
  • 1.5.2.1.1. Guinean Kpelle gkp
  • 1.5.2.1.2. Liberian Kpelle xpe
  • 1.5.2.1.3. Kono knu
  • 1.5.2.2. Looma
  • 1.5.2.2.1. Liberian Looma lom
  • 1.5.2.2.2. Guinean Looma (Toma) tod
  • 1.5.2.3. Bandi bza
  • 1.5.2.4. Zialo zil
  • 1.5.2.5. Mende men
  • 1.5.2.6. Loko lok
  • 1.6. Soninke–Bozo
  • 1.6.1. Soninke snk
  • 1.6.2. Bozo
  • 1.6.2.1. Bozo-Tigemaxo boz
  • 1.6.2.2. Kelengaxo bzx
  • 1.6.2.3. Jenaama bze
  • 1.6.2.4. Tiemacewe boo
  • 1.7. Bobo
  • 1.7.1. Southern Bobo bwq
  • 1.7.2. Konabere bbo
  • 1.8. Samogo
  • 1.8.1. Duun dux
  • 1.8.2. Dzuun dnn
  • 1.8.3. Kpan
  • 1.8.4. Banka bxw
  • 1.8.5. Seenku (Sembla) sos
  • 1.8.6. Jowulu jow
  • 2. South-Eastern Mande
  • 2.1. Southern Mande
  • 2.1.1. Beng nhb
  • 2.1.2. Ngen gnj
  • 2.1.3. Gbin
  • 2.1.4. Gban (Gagu) ggu
  • 2.1.5. Wan wan
  • 2.1.6. Guro (Kweni) goa
  • 2.1.7. Yaure yre
  • 2.1.8. Mwan (Mona) moa
  • 2.1.9. Dan (Yacouba, Gio) dnj
  • 2.1.9.1. Eastern Dan (Yacouba)
  • 2.1.9.2. Western Dan (Yacouba)
  • 2.1.9.3. Kla-Dan lda
  • 2.1.10. Mano mev
  • 2.1.11. Tura (Wen) neb
  • 2.1.12. Goo
  • 2.2. Eastern Mande
  • 2.2.1. San
  • 2.2.1.1. Southern San sbd
  • 2.2.1.2. San-Matya stj
  • 2.2.1.3. San-Maya sym
  • 2.2.2. Bisa bib
  • 2.2.3. Boko-Busa
  • 2.2.3.1. Boko bqc
  • 2.2.3.2. Bokobaru bus
  • 2.2.3.3. Busa bqp
  • 2.2.3.4. Illo-Busa
  • 2.2.4. Shanga-Kyenga
  • 2.2.4.1. Shanga sho
  • 2.2.4.2. Kyenga tye

1.10. Orthographic conventions

33All Mande languages are tonal. In our paper, tonal marking follows the IPA system. This system is illustrated in the Table 1, with the example of the vowel a.

Table 1. Tonal marking

Type of system →
Tone ↓
2-level system 3-level system 4-level system 5-level system
extra-high    
high
mid    
low
extra-low    

34If the tonal notation in a consulted source is different, it is converted to the IPA-based model here. Tonal notation in Bamana follows the rules formulated in (Konta & Vydrin 2014).

2. Sample studies: eight numeral systems representing different models

2.1. The modern and old Bamana systems

  • 8 Kalilou Tera, personal communication.

35In modern Bamana, the counting system is practically identical to those found in the majority of languages of the Manding group (Maninka, Jula), and it is consistently decimal, with relics of quinarity. However, another numeral system was predominant in Bamana till the mid-20th century, and it appears in colonial era grammars (Delafosse 1929; Molin 1956; Sauvant 1942). According to oral tradition,8 the archaic Bamana counting system was introduced by one of the rulers of the Segu kingdom which existed in the 17th-19th centuries and played a key role in the formation of the Bamana ethnic group. As late as the 1980-s, many Bamana speakers, especially natives of Segu, still mastered it, although they recognized that it was in the process of being abandoned and replaced by the decimal system. Nowadays, it is rarely used even by older generations, who, when questioned, often mix up features of the two systems or forget important details.

36In the old Bamana system, the numerals of the first ten are the same as in the modern system. The same is mainly true for the second ten.

Table 2. Bamana numerals, first and second ten

1

kélen

2

fìla

3

sàba

4

náani

5

dúuru

6

wɔ́ɔrɔ

7

wólonfìla, wólonfùla, wólonwùla

8

ségin, séegin

9

kɔ̀nɔntɔ, kɔ̀nɔntɔn

10

tán

11

tân ní kélen

 

(10+1)

12

tân ní fìla

 (10+2)

etc.

37The divergences begin at the end of the second ten, where subtractive numerals emerge. Table 2 presents various forms in the old and modern systems. In the left column, the numerical values are indicated; for the compound numerals, the syntax of the form is clarified in brackets. For numerical values which can be expressed in the old system in two ways (or more), the alternative forms are separated by a tilde (~).

Table 3. The old and modern Bamana systems

  Old system Modern systema

19

Mùgan kélen ká jɛ̀ ~ mùgan kélen ɲà (20-1) ~ tan ni kɔ̀nɔntɔn (10+9) tân ni kɔ̀nɔntɔn (10+9)

20

mùgan mùgan

21

mùgan ni kélen (20+1) mùgân ni kélen (20+1)

29

mùgan ni tàn kélen ká jɛ̀ ~ mùgan ní tàn kélen ɲà (20+10-1) mùgân ni kɔ̀nɔntɔn (20+9)

30

mùgan ni tán (20+10) bî sàba (10x3)

31

mùgan ni tán ni kélen (20+10+1) bî sàba ni kélen (10x3+1)

38

dɛ̀bɛ fìla ká jɛ̀ ~ dɛ̀bɛ fìla ɲà (40-2) bî sàba ni séegin (10x3+8)

40

dɛ̀bɛ bî náani (10x4)

50

dɛ̀bɛ ni tán (40+10) bî dúuru (10x5)

60

màninkɛ̀mɛ bî wɔ́ɔrɔ (10x6)

70

màninkɛ̀mɛ ni tán (60+10) ~ kɛ̀mɛ tán ɲà (80-10)~ kɛ̀mɛ tán ká jɛ̀ (80-10) bî wólonwùla (10x7)

75

màninkɛ̀mɛ ni tán ni dúuru (60+10+5) ~ kɛ̀mɛ dlá ɲà (80-1/2) bî wólonwùla ni dúuru (10x7+5)

80

kɛ̀mɛ ~ bámanankɛmɛ bî séegin (10x8)

90

kɛ̀mɛ ni tán (80+10) bî kɔ̀nɔntɔn (10x9)

100

kɛ̀mɛ ni mùgan (80+20) ~ sìlamɛyakɛmɛ kɛ̀mɛ

140

kɛ̀mɛ fìla mùgan ká jɛ̀ (80x2-20) kɛ̀mɛ ni bî náani (100+10x4)

160

kɛ̀mɛ fìla (80x2) kɛ̀mɛ ni bî wɔ́ɔrɔ (100+10x6)

200

kɛ̀mɛ fìla ni dɛ̀bɛ (80x2+40) kɛ̀mɛ fìla (100x2)

600

kɛ̀mɛ wólonwùla ni dɛ̀bɛ (80x7+40) ~ màninkaba kɛ̀mɛ wɔ́ɔrɔ (100x6)

800

bà kɛ̀mɛ séegin (100x8)

1000

bà kélen ni kɛ̀mɛ fìla ni dɛ̀bɛ (800+80x2+40)~ sìlamɛyaba kélen bà
a. The numerals for ‘10’ and ‘20’ in modern Bamana, when used in compound numerals which are not divisible by 10 and 20 respectively, appear with a postposed floating low tone; otherwise, they have no floating tone. This suggests that in this context they behave as nouns and therefore they likely carry the tonal article.

38The old Bamana system was characterized by a combination of the following counting systems:

  • quinary (within the first ten): in this, both old an new systems coincide. Quinarity manifests itself in the forms 6 wɔ́ɔrɔ and 7 wóronfìla. The latter form can be easily subdivided into two elements, wóron-fìla, where fìla is evidently identical with fìla ‘two’. The other form, wɔ́ɔrɔ ‘6’, is less transparent. Nonethess it could plausibly be subdivided into two elements, wɔ́ɔ-rɔ. In this case, it is highly probable that the second element comes from the reconstructed Proto-Mande numeral *dɔ́ ‘one’. In turn, the first element may be etymologically identical with the element wóron- attested in the numeral for ‘7’ (i.e., ‘6’ *wóron-dɔ > wɔ́ɔrɔ through elision of the foot-internal consonant, contraction of the two-feet structure into one foot with subsequent lenition *-d- -r- and regressive vocalic assimilation *-oo- > -ɔɔ-). It would be logical to suppose that wóron could be an archaic term for ‘five’;
  • heteroradical vigesimal (for tens). Numerals divisible by 20 (40, 60, 80, 100), contrary to expectations, are not formed through multiplication of the numeral for '20', they are represented instead by simple forms (dɛ̀bɛ '40', kɛ̀mɛ '80') or forms derived in idiosyncratic ways: màninkɛ̀mɛ can be interpreted as 'a Maninka hundred', bámanankɛmɛ is 'a Bamana hundred', and sìlamɛyakɛmɛ is 'a Muslim hundred';
  • mixed octogesimal and decimal, for hundreds and thousands (the numerical value bà ‘800’ is equal to 80x10).
  • In the old Bamana system, the following arithmetic operations were used:
  • addition, marked by the coordinative conjunction ni ‘and/with’ (tân ni kélen ‘11’ = 10+1),
  • multiplication, expressed by juxtaposition (kɛ̀mɛ fìla ‘160’ = 80 x 2),
  • subtraction (‘19’ = 20 – 1, mùgan kélen ɲà, lit. ‘twenty, make one lacking’, or mùgan kélen ká jɛ̀, lit. ‘twenty, let one be lacking’); the subtractable numerals are kélen 1, fìla 2, tán 10 and mùgan 20 (and also ‘half’ = 5). Subtractive numerals are employed in an imperative construction with the verb ɲà ‘lack; take away’ or a subjunctive construction with the verb jɛ̀ ‘miss, be absent; deprive’ (supposedly, preference for one verb or the other depends on the region);
  • elements of division: when forming subtractive numerals, ‘5’ can be designated as a ‘half of ten’: dɛ̀bɛ dlá ɲà ‘35’ (‘40 – a half’, lit. ‘forty, make a half lacking’). The word for ‘half’ is normally tíla, but in this context it appears with a voiced initial consonant (although, rarely, it may also be found with a voiceless consonant).

39The term kɛ̀mɛ appears in the numerals for 60, 80 and 100 which are etymologized as follows: màninkɛ̀mɛ ‘60’ lit. ‘a Maninka hundred’; bámanankɛmɛ ‘80’ lit. ‘a Bamana hundred’ (the simple stem, kɛ̀mɛ, can also be used for 80); sìlamɛyakɛmɛ ‘100’ lit. ‘a Muslim hundred’. 100 can also be designated by means of the “Bamana hundred”: kɛ̀mɛ ni mùgan (80 + 20). When kɛ̀mɛ is used in compound numerals, it designates, by default, ‘80’.

40As for the etymology of the numerals for tens, Maurice Delafosse proposed that mùgan ‘20’ could be traced back to the word mɔ̀gɔ ‘human being’ (for the number of a human's fingers and toes, cf. (Delafosse 1955: 520)), and dɛ̀bɛ ‘40’ to dɛ̀bɛn ‘mat’ (for the number of fingers and toes of a couple lying on a mat, cf. (Delafosse 1929: 273–274; Delafosse 1955: 111)). These etymologies may be correct (especially for mɔ̀gɔ, much less clearly for dɛ̀bɛ), all the more so given that irregular form modifications accompany numeral derivations from nouns in many African languages.

41The stem kɛ̀mɛ ‘100/80’ (and various forms which can be traced back to it) is well represented in Mande languages, and there are good reasons to think that it spread through intra-African trade, and primarily the trade in cola nuts. One hypothesis is that it designated a standard cola package (which could vary regionally, leading to different meanings for the “Bambara hundred” and “Maninka hundred” in the old Bamana counting system). Alternatively, kɛ̀mɛ could originally stand for a string of cowrie shells, the predominant pre-colonial currency. Taking into account the role of Soninke which was the dominant language of Ghana (the very first among the medieval empires of Western Sudan), the language of the jùla traders and the source of numerous loans in the sphere of cultural vocabulary in many languages of the region, it can be supposed that the ultimate origin of this stem could be Soninke, where it appears in the forms kàmé (in Western dialects) and kèmé (in Eastern dialects).

42The etymology of the word bà (‘800’ in the old system, ‘1000’ in the modern system) is not clear. It is noteworthy that in Bamana, there is a special word for counting money, wáa ‘1000’ (while in Guinean Maninka and in interethnic Jula, wáa is the general word for ‘1000’) which goes back to the noun wága ~ wáa ‘basket for cola nuts’ (storing 1000 nuts). This stem also appears in many other Mande languages.

  • 9 The existence of these forms is confirmed by less regular data from the field notes (1887-1889) of (...)
  • 10 Most probably, toko is a Senufo form in origin, cf. tòkoo ‘20’ in Tyebara Senufo (Mills 2003: 119 (...)

43The basic unit for money counting in Bamana is dɔ́rɔ̌mɛ́ ‘5 CFA francs’ (less frequently, dálasi, idem.), cf. in more detail 1.5. The existence of a special money count word wáa ‘1000’ (= 5000 CFA francs) should also be mentioned. Maurice Delafosse (Delafosse 1929: 280–281) mentions the existence of special terms for counting cowrie shells which were the main currency in the Manding area during the pre-colonial period (these words are no longer used and we therefore reproduce Delafosse's spelling of the forms since we cannot verify their pronunciation):9 dã ‘5 cowries’, daba ‘10 cowries’, toko ‘20 cowries’,10 sīra ‘200 cowries’. These words are always accompanied by regular numerals (toko kele ni dã kele ‘25 cowries’, toko fila ‘40 cowries’, etc.) and should therefore be classified as nouns.

44There are several counting wods in Bamana: cɛ̀ ‘man’, mùso ‘woman’, dén ‘child’, ‘fruit’, kílo ‘kilogram’, ɲɛ́ ‘content’, kìsɛ ‘grain’ (used for counting small objects), kùru ‘ball, lump’ (used for counting round objects), kàla ‘stem’ (used to count long objects). They are not yet fully grammaticalized as classifiers and belong to the part of speech of noun.

45External syntax of Bamana numerals. Numerals follow the nouns they determine, and by default, neither noun, nor numeral has plural marker –u (-w in the current orthography). This marker can nevertheless be added to the numeral (1) or to the noun (2) (seemingly without any distinction) to express definiteness.

(1) Npògotigi` ka kán kà dí ́nnì mà, ̀ sàba-w ni ɲɔ́gɔn ́?
girl-art qual.aff equal inf give who adr man three-pl and recp between
‘From among the three men, to whom should the girl be given?’

46 

(2) <…> ù màa-w fìla ́ɛ ̀-ra kà Lànseni fò, à ye ù lámì.
3pl human-pl two all gather-pfv.intr inf Lanseni greet 3sg pfv.tr 3pl answer
‘... both of them gathered to greet Lanseni, and he responded to their greeting’.

2.2. Boko numeral system

  • 11 In this book, forms for numerals are often presented with variable tones, without any explanation (...)

47Boko (Eastern Mande) is spoken on both sides of the border between Nigeria and Benin. It is distant from the zone of Manding and Soninke dominance, which means that these two languages are unlikely donors of cultural vocabulary for Boko. The data on the Boko numerative system are given according to (Jones 1998; Jones 2004),11 cf. Table 4.

48Boko is one of the rare languages in the Mande family where the quinary system continues beyond the first ten since the numerals for ‘15’ and ‘25’ serve as augends (for 16, 18, 26, 27) and minuends (for 14, 24). Otherwise, vigesimality is predominant in Boko.

49Basic numerals. There are non-compound terms (at least, in synchrony) for the numerals from 1 to 5, and also ‘10’ kwȉ; ‘15’ gɛ̰̏ó; ‘20’ bȁó; ‘200’ ɔ̏áȁ.

50In the forms for 6, 7, 8, there is an element sOO- which can be traced back to sɔ̋ó ‘5’. In 6 sɔ́ólɔ́, the element –lɔ́ derives, most probably, from dó ‘1’. In the numerals for 7 and 8, the final elements are identical with the numerals for 2 and 3. Therefore, the numerals of the first ten retain elements of the ancient quinary counting system. In the form kɛ̃́ókwȉ ‘9’, the element kwȉ ‘10’ is easily detectable. The initial element can be subdivided into -ó- which most likely goes back to dó ‘one’, and kɛ̰́ which can be linked to the verb kɛ̃̋ ‘tear, break’. If this etymology is correct, kɛ̃́ókwȉ is a subtractive numeral (10 – 1), lit. ‘break one (from) ten’.

Table 4. Boko numerals

1 dó
2 plá
3 ȁȁó̰
4 si̋i̋ɔ̰́
5 sɔ̋ó
6 sɔ́ólɔ́ (5+1)
7 sóplá (5+2)
8 swȁȁṍ (5+3)
9 kɛ̰́ókwȉ (–1 10)
10 kwí
11 kúɛ̏dó (10+1)
12 kúɛ̏plá (10+2)
13 kúɛ̏ ’ȁȁɔ̰́ (10+3)
14 gɛ̰̏ó mɛ̏ŋ̏ dó sȁi̋ (15-1)
15 gɛ̰̏ó ((*foot)x1)
16 gɛ̰̏ó ŋ̋ mɛ̏ŋ̏ dó-ȍ (15+1)
17 gɛ̰̏ó ŋ̋ plá ȍ (15+2)
18 bȁó plá sȁi̋ ((20x1)–2)
19 bȁó dó sȁi̋ ((20x1)–1)
20 bȁó (20x1)
21 bȁó ŋ̋ plá ȍ ((20x1)+1)
23 bȁó ŋ̋ ȁȁɔ̰́ ȍ ((20x1)+3)
24 bȁásɔ̋ó dó sȁi̋ ((20x1)+5)–1)
25 bȁásɔ̋ó ((20x1)+5)
26 bȁásɔ̋ó ŋ̋ mɛ̏ŋ̏ dó ȍ (((20x1)+5)+1 thing)
27 bȁásɔ̋ó ŋ̋ plá ȍ (((20x1)+5)+2)
28 bȁá kwȉ plá sȁi̋ (20+10–2)
30 bȁá kwȉ (20+10)
35 sómȁ b̏lá ~ sóȁ b̏lá ~ sɔ̰́ȁ b̏lá (–5 40)
36 bȁá kwȉ ŋ̋ sɔ́ólɔ́ ȍ ((20+10)+(5+1))
37 bȁá kwȉ ŋ̋ sóplá ȍ ((20+10)+(5+2))
38 b̏lá plá sȁi̋ ((20x2) – 2)
39 b̏lá dó sȁi̋ ((20x2) – 1)
40 b̏lá (20x2)
45 b̏lá sɔ̋ó ((20x2)+5)
50 b̏lá kwȉ (40+10)
55 sómȁ bȁ ȁȁɔ̰́ (–5 (20x3))
56 b̏lá kwȉ ŋ̋ sɔ́ólɔ́ ȍ ((40+10)+(5+1))
57 b̏lá kwȉ ŋ̋ sóplá ȍ ((40+10)+(5+2))
58 bȁ ȁȁṍ plá sȁi̋ ((20x3)–2)
60 bȁ ȁȁṍ (20x3)
65 bȁ ȁȁṍ sɔ̋ó ((20x3)+5)
70 bȁ ȁȁṍ kwȉ ((20x3)+10)
80 bȁ si̋i̋ɔ̰́ (20x4)
85 bȁ si̋i̋ɔ̰́ sɔ̋ó ((20x4)+5)
90 bȁ si̋i̋ɔ̰́ kwȉ ((20x4)+10)
100 bȁ sɔ̋ó (20x5)
105 bȁ sɔ̋ó sɔ̋ó ((20x5)+5)
120 bȁ sőóló (20x6)
140 bȁ sóplá (20x7)
160 bȁ swȁȁṍ (20x8)
180 bȁ kɛ́ó̰kwȉ (20x9)
200 ‘ɔ̏áȁ dó (200x1)
300 ‘ɔ̏áȁ dó ŋ̋ bȁ sɔ̋ó ((200x1) + (20x5))
400 ‘ɔ̏áȁ plá (200x2)
500 ‘ɔ̏áȁ plá ŋ̋ bȁ sɔ̋ó ((200x1) + (20x5))
600 ‘ɔ̏áȁ 'ȁȁṍ (200x3)
700 ‘ɔ̏áȁ 'ȁȁṍ ŋ̋ bȁ sɔ̋ó ((200x2) + (20x5))
800 ‘ɔ̏áȁ si̋i̋ó (200x4)
900 ‘ɔ̏áȁ si̋i̋ó ŋ̋ bȁ sɔ̋ó ((200x4) + (20x5))
501 ‘ɔ̏áȁ plá ŋ́ bȁ sɔ̋ó ŋ́ mɛ̏ŋ̏ dó ȍ ((200x2) + (20x5) + 1 thing)
750 ‘ɔ̏áȁ 'ȁȁṍ ŋ́ bȁ sóplá kwȉ ȍ ((200x3) + (20x7) + 10)
188 ‘ɔ̏áȁ dó kúɛ̏plá sȁi̋ ((200x1) – 12)
1000 ‘ɔ̏áȁ sɔ̋ó (200x5)
1400 ‘ɔ̏áȁ sóplá (200x7)
2944 ‘ɔ̏áȁ gɛ̰̏ó dó sȁi̋ ŋ̋ bȁ sóplá ŋ̋ si̋i̋ɔ̰́ ȍ (200 x (15-1) + (20x7) + 4)
50,000 ɔ̏áȁ sɔ̋sɔ̋ó lɛ́ɛ̏ b̏lá kwí ((200x5redupl. part) (40+10))

51The word for ‘10’ kwȉ can be (very tentatively) linked to the stem for ‘hand’. In modern Boko, ‘hand’ is ’ɔ́, and a preliminary reconstruction for this root at the Proto-Eastern level is *gɔ̌n (cf. Proto-Southern Mande *kɔ̀ ‘hand’ and forms for ‘10’ in Dan, Goo, Tura).

52There are good reasons to think that the stem gɛ̰̏ó ‘15’ may contain an element originally meaning ‘foot’: although the word for ‘foot/leg’ in Boko is gbá, a tentative pre-reconstruction for Proto-Eastern Mande is *gɔ̰̀ or *gwɛ̰̀ ‘foot’, cf. Henning Schreiber's (2008: 323) Proto-Eastern reconstruction *gwaN ‘foot’, cf. also Southern San gòã (Anonym 2003), Lebir gǎm |gànɩ́| (Vanhoudt 1999). The final element -ó in ‘15’ may go back to dó ‘1’, i.e. the term originally meant ‘(two hands and) one foot’, this model for ‘15’ is not exceptional in African languages (Pozdniakov 2018: 120).

53The basic term of the vigesimal system bȁ- (presumably, from bà ‘rope, string’, i.e. ‘string of cowrie shells’) appears in the form bȁó ‘20’, where the final element -ó may go back to dó ‘one’; in bȁásɔ̋ó ‘25’ (cf. sɔ̋ó ‘5’, while the etymology of the linker -á- is unclear); bȁákwȉ ‘30’ (kwȉ < kwí ‘10’; the same linker -á- as in ‘25’); 40 b̏lá (a contractive form of *bȁ ‘20’ + plá ‘2’), and in other numerals divisible by 20.

54An etymology for ɔ̏áȁ ‘200’ may be suggested if we compare this form with those seen in the closely related languages Bokobaru, wàà, and Busa, wàà. It cannot be confirmed that these forms are related to the forms of the type WAA, WAGA for ‘1000’ which are wide spread in Mande languages (usually with high tones), but it can be accepted as a potential hypothesis.

55For counting from 20 through 2000, the vigesimal system is applied quite consisently: 900 ‘ɔ̏áȁ si̋i̋ɔ̰́ ŋ̋ bȁ sɔ̋ó (200х4 + 20х5).

56Arithmetic operations, linkers and inner morphosyntax of compound numerals in Boko.

57In addition, there are certain limitations on the units which can serve as addends. In particular, in the first, second and third tens, ‘4’ si̋i̋ó does not appear in this function, the relevant meanings being expressed via subtraction; ‘6’ and ‘7’ are not used to form ‘16’ and ‘17’, ‘26’ and ‘27’, the formulae “15+1”, “15+2”, “(20+5)+1”, “(20+5)+2” being preferred. By contrast, these units do appear in the compound numerals above 30.

58There are different means used to express addition in the formation of numerals. Juxtaposition is used for the lower compound numerals ‘6’, ‘7’, ‘8’ (the model “5+X”) and higher ones divisible by 5: ‘25’, ‘45’ (the model “20xN+5); ‘30’, ‘50’, ‘70’, ‘90’ (the model “20xN+10”). Juxtaposition is accompanied by fusion: the stems for the augends ‘5’ (in ‘6’, ‘7’, ‘8’) and ‘10’ (in ‘11’, ‘12’, ‘13’) are modified: ‘5’ sɔ́ó só- or sw-; ‘10’ kwí kúɛ̏. In ‘6’, the addend stem is also modified: ‘1’ dó -lɔ́. For higher numerals, there is no fusion, however, the addend ‘10’ changes its tone: kwí kwȉ.

59For numerals higher than 15 and not divisible by 5 (and not formed by subtraction, see below), addition is expressed by the comitative frame construction ŋ̋ … ȍ ‘with’. To add 1, one must use the numerative word mɛ̏ŋ̏: bȁ sɔ̋ó ŋ̋ mɛ̏ŋ̏ dó ȍ (20x5+1 unit) ‘101’.

  • 12 Therefore, Boko violates Greenberg's 12th (1990: 279) universal: “A subtrahend is always a simple (...)

60Subtraction in Boko is comparable to addition in terms of its productivity. In the second and third fives, the subtrahend is ‘1’; for higher numerals, it can be ‘1’ and ‘2’ (for all minuends divisible by 5, i.e. “5xN–1 or 2”, N > 3), or ‘5’ (for minuends divisible by 20, beginning from 40, i.e. “20xN–5”, N > 1). For the hundreds (with minuend ‘ɔ̏áȁ ‘200’ or higher), the subtrahend can range between 1 and 25.12

61There are three formal means for producing subtractive numerals.

62The most prevalent is the following: the subtrahend follows the minuend and is followed by the postposition sȁi̋ ‘without’ (the subtrahend dó ‘1’ can be preceded by the numerative word mɛ̏ŋ̏):

(3) gɛ̰̏ó (mɛ̏ŋ̏) dó sȁi̋
15 unit 1 without
‘14’
 
(4) ‘ɔ̏áȁ dó kúɛ̏-plá sȁi̋
200 1 10-2 without
‘188’ = 200x1 – (10 – 2)

63In order to avoid repetition of the postposition sȁi̋ within one compound numeral (when a subtrahend itself contains a subtrahend), the following formula is used:

(5) ɔ̏áȁ dó gɛ̰̏ó dó sȁi̋ mɛ̋ kú-ó
200 1 15 1 without foc here-neg
‘186’ = 200x1 – (15–1)
  • 13 However, according to Ross Jones (p.c.), sómȁ is an archaic word; younger people use “30+5” for (...)

64If the subtrahend is 5 (for the minuends divisible by 20, beginning from 40), a special word sómȁ ‘–5’ (with phonetic variants sóȁ, sɔ̰́ȁ, súȁ, sṵ́ȁ) is used. This word precedes the minuend: sómȁ b̏lá ‘35’, sómȁ bȁ ȁȁɔ̰́ ‘55’.13 The etymology of this word is unclear, although one may speculate that the initial element só- might be related to sɔ̋ó ‘5’. This model may seem to violate 10th Greenberg's generalization (1990: 278–279): “Subtraction is never expressed by the mere sequence of the subtrahend and minuend”. However, this is not a clear-cut case, since sómà is not a regular numeral, but rather a specialized subtrahend form (i.e., it is somewhat similar to the Tarahumara case cited by Greenberg).

65The third means of formation is applied to only one subtractive numeral, kɛ̰́ó kwȉ ‘9’, lit. ‘cut-one ten’.

66Multiplication is used for higher numerals. Only two basic numerals, bȁ ‘20’ and ɔ̏áȁ ‘200’, can function as multiplicands; the multiplication is expressed by juxtaposition (the multiplier follows the multiplicand), e.g. ‘ɔ̏áȁȁȁó̰ (200х3) ‘600’. One complex numeral (itself formed through multiplication), ‘ɔ̏áȁ sɔ̋ó (200x5) ‘1000’, can appear as a multiplicand, in which case its second component undergoes partial reduplication, and the multiplier is preceded by the linker lɛ́ɛ̏ ‘part, group’: ɔ̏áȁ sɔ̋sɔ̋ó lɛ́ɛ̏ plá (200x5redupl. part 2) ‘2000’; ɔ̏áȁ sɔ̋sɔ̋ó lɛ́ɛ̏ ɔ̏áȁ sɔ̋ó (200x5redupl. part 200x5) ‘100,000’.

67In Boko, there are three classifiers (numerative words) which may appear when numerals are used as quantifiers: gbɛ̰́ɔ̰́ŋ́, for counting humans; mɛ̏ŋ̏, for other objects; and gɛ̰̏ŋ̏ ‘time’, for actions. The rules of their use and their degree of obligatoriness are not discussed by Jones (1998: 128–129).

(6) Má pɔ̋ bɛ̰̋ɛ̰̋ mɛ̏ŋ̏ si̋i̋ɔ̰́ pì-ɔ́ ‘ȅ.
1sg.pfv thing alive object four art-pl see.pfv
‘I saw these four living things’.

2.3. Numerals in Dzuungoo

68Dzuungoo (Samogo group, Western Mande) is spoken in western Burkina Faso. The numerals of this language are represented in Table 5 according to (Solomiac 2014: 152–158).

69In Dzuungoo, elements of a quinary system are practically absent: the only potential survival is seen in the form ɲɛ̀ɛ̀nún ‘7’, in which the element nún may be etymologically connected to nùn ‘5’. This hypothesis, however, is contradicted by its final position in the form (everywhere else in Dzuungoo, just as in all other Mande languages, the larger addend precedes the smaller one, without any exception).

Table 5. Dzuungoo numerals

1

sōō, sɔ̄n

2

fíí

3

zhììgī

4

nààlén

5

nùn

6

tsùnmɛ̄n

7

ɲɛ̀ɛ̀nún

8

ɲáálòn

9

kyèèrón

10

tsyéù

11

téēŋsɔ̄n (10+1)

12

téēŋfíí (10+2)

13

téēŋzhíígī (10+3)

14

téeŋnáàlén (10+4)

15

téēŋnún (10+5)

16

téēŋtsúnmɛ̄n (10+6)

17

téēŋɲɛ́ɛ̀nún (10+7)

18

téēŋáálòn (10+8)

19

téēŋkyéèrón (10+9)

20

mòò

21

móòsɔ̄n (20+1)

22

móòfíí (20+2)

23

móòzhììgī (20+3)

28

móòŋáálòn (20+8)

29

móòkyèèrón (20+9)

30

mòòtsyéù (20+10)

31

mòòtséù kó (dzín) sóó ((20+10)+1)

32

mòòtséù kó (dzín) fíí ((20+10)+2)

40

dzyèè

50

dzyèètsyéù (40+10)

60

mùéy

70

mùéy kó tséù (60+10)

80

cɛ̀ɛ̀n (as determiner), cɛ̀ɛ̀nsōō (in counting)

90

cɛ̀ɛ̀n kó tsyéù (80+10)

100

càànmóò (80+20)

110

càànmóò kó tsyéù ((80+20)+10)

120

cààndzyéè (80+40)

130

cààndzyéè kó tsyéù ((80+40)+10)

140

cɛ̀ɛ̀nfíímòòyáá ((80x2) – 20)

160

cɛ̀ɛ̀nfíí (80x2)

180

cɛ̀ɛ̀nfíí kó mòò

200

cànfóōdzyéè (80x2 + 40)

400

cɛ̀ɛ̀nún (80x5)

800

gúrúsɔ̄n (800x1)

1000

gúrúsɔ̄n kó cànfóōdzyéè (800x1 + ((80x2) + 40)), or wāākērēn, or gbáásōō

1200

ɲɛ̄yn shē zhììgī (cowrie string 3)

  • 14 For ‘80’ some speakers use an extended form, bāmāā náá cɛ̀ɛ̀n, lit. ‘Senufo's 80’, which corr (...)
  • 15 In fact, no phonological reconstruction for Proto-Mande (or even the majority of the lower-level b (...)

70Basic numerals. Apart from the numerals of the first ten, there are simple forms for the following numbers: 10 tsyéù, 20 mòò, 40 dzyèè, 60 mùéí, 80 cɛ̀ɛ̀n, 800 gúrúsɔ̄n. The numeral ‘20’ mòò is a full homonym of the noun for ‘human being’. Therefore, while in several other Mande languages the etymological development ‘human being’ > ‘20’ (the number of fingers and toes of a human) is somewhat blurred (e.g., Bamana mɔ̀gɔ ‘human being’ vs. mùgan ‘20’, Susu mìxí ‘human being’ vs. mɔ̀xɔ́ɲɛ̀ń ‘20’), in Dzuungoo it is straightforward. The forms for ‘40’ and ‘80’ may be borrowed from the Old Bamana system (dɛ̀bɛ and kɛ̀mɛ respectively),14 and the same origin cannot be excluded for ‘60’ (cf. Old Bamana màninkɛmɛ). The term for 800 gúrúsɔ̄n can be subdivided into gúrú which may be etymologically identical to forms found widely in Western Mande for 1000 (Bozo-Tigemaxo gulu,15 Mandinka wúlî ~ wílî, Loko wul, etc.), and sɔ̄n which goes back to sōō ‘1’.

71The numerals of the second ten contain the stem téēŋ-, which might be connected with the numeral 10 tsyéù.

72The counting system from 20 through 80 is based on the heteroradical vigesimal principle, and above 80 it is taken over by an octogesimal model. The element cààn ~ càn, which regularly appears in compound numerals (100 (80+20), 110 (80+20+10), 120 (80+40), 130 (80+40+10), 200 (80х2+40) etc.), is formally distinct from the numeral cɛ̀ɛ̀n ‘80’, but they are, without any doubt, etymologically related. In one numeral, cɛ̀ɛ̀nfíí ‘160’ (80x2), the original form cɛ̀ɛ̀n ‘80’ appears unmodified.

73When counting thousands (and higher), Dzuun people cross over to a decimal system and use only the Jula loan wāākērēn (< wáa kélen), or the Dzuun calque gbáásōō. The expression ɲɛ̄yn shē ‘cowrie string’ is used as a base with the numeral value ‘400’ only in the compound numeral ‘1200’.

74Arithmetic operations, linkers and inner morphosyntax of compound numerals in Dzuungoo

75Addition. There are three formal models of addition:

76a) through fusion of the summands, with one or both components undergoing modification in the great majority of cases. This model is applied to the numerals:

  • from 11 through 19, which are accompanied by both segmental and suprasegmental modification of the stem ‘10’ (tsyéù téēŋ-) and by tonal rising on the first syllable of the component for the units (zhììgī zhíígī ‘3’, kyèèrón kyéèrón ‘5’, etc.). The numeral ‘11’ is an exception, because the name of the unit is modified segmentally, and not tonally (sōō sɔ̄n). Solomiac's hypothesis is that the final element –ŋ of the form téēŋ- may go back to the connector *kó whose high tone may also have been retained as a floating high tone at the end of téēŋ-;
  • from 21 through 29, where the smaller addend is not modified, while the name of the larger summand (the augend) is tonally modified (mòò móò- ‘20’). Solomiac attributes this to the omission of the connector *kó;
  • ‘100’ (80+20) and ‘120’ (80+40), where the first summand is segmentally modified (cɛ̀ɛ̀n cààn- ‘80’), and the second one raises its initial tone. In the numeral cànfóōdzyéè ‘200’ (80x2 + 40), the first addend, although complex, is segmentally modified (cɛ̀ɛ̀nfíí ‘160’ → cànfóō-), while the second one maintains its form.

77b) Through simple juxtaposition of the summands (without any formal modification of the components), for the numerals ‘30’, ‘50’.

78c) With the connector kó ‘and’ between the addends, the addends are not modified in form. This model is used for linking tens and units in numbers higher than 30 (31 = (20+10)+1, 32 = (20+10)+2, etc.; 110 = (80+20)+10, 130 = (80+40)+10), but also for ‘70’ (60+10) and ‘90’ (80+10) where both addends are simple. When the second (smaller) addend is a unit, it can be preceded by the word dzín meaning ‘unit’ (original meaning: ‘child, fruit, grain’). Addition with kó is also used for higher numerals, e.g.:

(7) cànfóōdzyéè kó cɛ̀ɛ̀nfíímòòyáá kó sóó
80x2+40 and 80x2-20 and 1
‘341’ (200+141)
 
(8) cɛ̀ɛ̀nnún kó cànfóōdzyéè kó cɛ̀ɛ̀nfíímòòyáá kó sóó
80x5 and 80x2+40 and 80x2-20 and 1
‘741’ (400+200+141)

79Subtraction in compound numerals is limited in its use. In fact, the only attested case is represented by the numeral for 140 derived from 160 through subtraction (by means of the marker yáá ‘without’): cɛ̀ɛ̀n-fíí-mòò-yáá 140 (80х2-20).

  • 16 In an unpublished list of Dzuungoo numerals (courtesy of Paul Solomiac), the numeral for 400 is sp (...)

80Multiplication is applied to only one base, ‘80’ (to be more precise, the data are missing for numerals above 1200) and is expressed by simple juxtaposition of the components, the multiplicand cɛ̀ɛ̀n ‘80’ preceding the multiplier. Multiplication in Dzuungoo produces only two forms, cɛ̀ɛ̀n fíí ‘160’ (without any modification of the components) and cɛ̀ɛ̀nún ‘400’16 (the second component raises its tone). Contrary to expectations, the numerals for ‘240’, ‘320’, ‘580’ etc. are not formed by means of multiplication, but by addition, where cɛ̀ɛ̀nfíímòòyáá ‘200’ = 80x2+40 and cɛ̀ɛ̀nún ‘400’ = 80x5 appear as augends (cf. examples (7), (8)), which can be interpreted as evidence for evolution toward a mixed decimal-vigesimal system.

81The study of higher numerals in Dzuungoo is complicated by the fact that practically all speakers are bilingual in Jula, and most often they count in Dzuungoo up to 29 and then switch into Jula (Solomiac 2014: 152).

82Besides the traditional compound numeral gúrúsɔ̄n kó cànfóōdzyéè (800+80x2+40) following the octogesimal model, 1000 can be designated in the modern language by the Jula loan wāākērēn or by the better-integrated form gbáá (sōō) ‘(one) thousand’. Thus, elements of a decimal model have begun to penetrate the numeral system of Dzuungoo.

83External syntax of numerals

84Numerals follow the nouns they determine. In this construction, as a rule, the noun cannot carry any other determiner, and the numeral has none either. More rarely, the numeral can have a plural marker or a definite article, both of which express the same meaning of definiteness.

2.4. Numerals in Mwan

85Mwan (South Mande < Southeastern Mande) is spoken by some 17,000 people in the central part of Côte d’Ivoire. The data were collected by Elena Perekhvalskaya during her fieldwork. See also (Perekhvalskaya 2017: 726–727).

Table 6. Mwan numerals

1 dō
2 plɛ̄
3 yāgā
4 yīzīɛ̄
5 sóó
6 slɔ́ádō (5+1)
7 slɔ́áplɛ̄ (5+2)
8 slɔ́áā (5+3)
9 slɔ́áyīzīɛ̄ (5+4)
10 vū
11 vū tā do (10+1)
12 vū tā plɛ̄ (10+2)
13 vū tā yāgā (10+3)
20 mīā dō (20x1)
22 mīā dō ɓɛ̄ plɛ̄ (20x1 + 2)
30 mīā dō ɓɛ̄ vū (20x1 + 10)
35 mīā dō ɓɛ̄ vū tā sóó (20x1 + 10 + 5)
40 mīā plɛ̄ (20x2)
50 mīā plɛ̄ ɓɛ̄ vū (20x2 + 10)
57 mīā plɛ̄ ɓɛ̄ vū tā slɔ́áplɛ̄ (20x2 + 10 + (5+2))
60 mīā yāgā (20x3)
70 mīā yāgā ɓɛ̄ vū (20x3 + 10)
79 mīā yāgā ɓɛ̄ vū tā slɔ́áyīziɛ̄ (20x3 + 10 + 9 (= 5+4))
100 kɛ̀mɛ̀ dō (100x1)
120 kɛ̀mɛ̀ dō mīā dō (100 + 20)
140 kɛ̀mɛ̀ dō ké mīā plɛ̄ (100x1 + 20x2)
200 kɛ̀mɛ̀ plɛ̄ (100x2)
248 kɛ̀mɛ̀ plɛ̄ mīā plɛ̄ ɓɛ̄ slɔ́ā (100х2 + 20х2+8 (= 5+3))
338 kɛ̀mɛ̀ yāgā ké mīā dō ɓɛ̄ vū tā slɔ́á (100x3 + 20x1 + 10 + 8 (= 5+3))
565 kɛ̀mɛ̀ sóó mīā yāgā ɓɛ̄ sóó (100х5 + 20х3+5)
787 kɛ̀mɛ̀ slɔ́áplɛ̄ mīā yīzīɛ̄ ɓɛ̄ slɔ́áplɛ̄ (100х7 + 20х4+7 (= 5+2))
1000 wáá dō (1000x1)
2400 wáá plɛ̄ kɛ̄mɛ̄ yīzīɛ̄

86The numeral system of Mwan combines vigesimal (in the first hundred) and decimal (above 100) models. The numeral bases are mīā ‘20’, kɛ̀mɛ̀ ‘100’ and wáá ‘1000’. Obviously, the decimal model should be regarded as predominant in Mwan, while the elements of the vigesimal model may have been borrowed from the neighbouring Guro language (cf. Table 10). It should be noted that the Mwan and Guro communities are connected by intense commercial and marital ties; Guro speakers are much more numerous, and many Mwan speak some Guro. However, since the forms of the numerals for 20 in Mwan (mīā) and Guro (yọ̄) are not related, we can speak of borrowing of the model, but not of the forms (structural borrowing, in Haspelmath's (2009) sense).

87The other augends are sóó ‘5’ and vū ‘10’, both of Proto-Mande origin. We can speculate that the Mwan word for 20, mīā, might be etymologically related to the word mɛ̄ɛ̄ ‘human’. Kɛ̀mɛ̀ and wáá have most probably been borrowed from Manding.

88Addition. Several models of addition are used:

  • for the numerals ‘6–9’, the model is slɔ́-á-N. The element slɔ́- certainly stands for ‘5’ and provides evidence for the reconstruction of a word-internal consonant in this numeral, *sóló (lost in the independent numeral sóó ‘5’). The second element, -á- goes back, most probably, to the superessive postposition tā. The third element is the second addend, which may be fused (slɔ́ ‘8’) or not (slɔ́ádō ‘6’, slɔ́áplɛ̄ ‘7’, slɔ́áyīzīɛ̄ ‘9’);
  • for numerals above 10, the connector for the names of tens and units is a full-fledged superessive postposition tā;
  • for those numerals from 30 onwards divisible by 10 but not divisible by 20, the model is mīā N ɓɛ̄ vū (i.e., 20 N ɓɛ̄ 10, or “20 N + 10”). The connector ɓɛ̄ goes back to the noun ɓɛ̄ ‘grain’, this word is also used in singulative meaning (‘single object out of a mass or pair’: ylɛ̀ ɓɛ̄ dō ‘one eye’, zlɔ̀ ɓɛ̄ ‘a bee’);
  • for numerals above 100, ké (a comitative preposition) serves to connect names of hundreds and names of tens.

89Multiplication is expressed through simple juxtaposition of the multiplicand and the multiplier. The multiplicands in Mwan are mīā ‘20’, kɛ̀mɛ̀ ‘100’, wáá ‘1000’.

90No subtraction is used in the formation of compound numerals.

91Money counting has the same features as in numerous other languages of West Africa: when combined with the noun gɔ̀lí ‘money; 5 franc coin’ (or even in its absence, when money is meant), a numeral designates a quantity multiplied by five: gɔ̀lí kɛ̀mɛ̀ dō ‘500 francs’ (lit. ‘100 5-franc coins’); wáá yīzīɛ̄ ‘20,000’ (lit. ‘4 thousands’).

92External syntax. Numerals follow the determined noun which appears without the plural marker (mū): mɛ̄ɛ̄ plɛ̄ ‘two people’.

2.5. Dan-Gwɛɛtaa numerals

93Dan-Gwɛɛtaa is one of the numerous varieties of Dan (Southern Mande) spoken in the west of Côte d’Ivoire. The Dan-Gwɛɛtaa data were collected by Valentin Vydrin during his fieldwork; also see (Vydrin 2017).

Table 7. Dan-Gwɛɛtaa numerals

1 dō
2 plɛ̀ ~ pȅȅdʌ̄
3 yȁȁgā
4 yȉȉsīɤ̄
5 sɔ̋ɔ̋ɗű
6 sɒ̋ɒ̏dō (5+1)
7 sɒ̋ɒ̏plɛ̀ (5+2)
8 sa̋ȁgā (5+3)
9 sűȅsīɤ̄ (5+4)
10 kɒ̏ɒ̏ŋ̏ dō (10x1)
11 kɒ̏ɒ̏ŋ̏ dō ɤ̄ gā dō (10x1 + 1)
12 kɒ̏ɒ̏ŋ̏ dō ɤ̄ gā plɛ̀ (10x1 + 2)
13 kɒ̏ɒ̏ŋ̏ dō ɤ̄ gā yȁȁgā (10x1 + 3)
20 kɒ̏ɒ̏ŋ̏ plɛ̀ (10x2)
21 kɒ̏ɒ̏ŋ̏ plɛ̀ ɤ̄ gā dō (10x2 + 1)
22 kɒ̏ɒ̏ŋ̏ plɛ̀ ɤ̄ gā plɛ̀ ~ pȅȅdʌ̄ (10x2 +2)
30 kɒ̏ɒ̏ŋ̏ yȁȁgā (10x3)
40 kɒ̏ɒ̏ŋ̏ yȉȉsīɤ̄ (10x4)
90 kɒ̏ɒ̏ŋ̏ sűȅsīɤ̄ (10x9)
100 kʌ̄ŋ̄ dō (100x1)
102 kʌ̄ŋ̄ dō wāȁ plɛ̏ (100x1 and 2)
111 kʌ̄ŋ̄ dō wāȁ kɒ̏ɒ̏ŋ̏ dō wāȁ dō (100x1 and 10x1 and 1) ~ kʌ̄ŋ̄ dō ɤ̄ kɔ̏ dō wāȁ dō (100x1 its hand x1 and 1)
200 kʌ̄ŋ̄ plɛ̀ (100x2)
1000 gblɯ̋ dō (1000x1)
2000 gblɯ̋ plɛ̀ (1000x2)
2222 gblɯ̋ plɛ̀ wāȁ kʌ̄ŋ̄ plɛ̀ ɤ̄ kɔ̏ plɛ̀ wāȁ ɤ̄ gā plɛ̀ (1000x2 and 100x2 its hand x2 and its bone x2)
10,000 gblɯ̋ kɒ̏ɒ̏ŋ̏ dō (1000x10x1)
100,000 gblɯ̋ kʌ̄ŋ̄ dō (1000x100x1)
1,000,000 gblɯ̋ ɓɛ̃̄ɛ̃̏ dō
  • 17 In fact, gblɯ̋ in Dan may be etymologically related to the root WULU ~ WULI ~ GULU attested in man (...)

94In Dan-Gwɛɛtaa, a quinary model appears in the numerals from 6 through 9, although it is obscured by fusion: comparison with other South Mande languages allows us to reconstruct the model “5-tȁ-1/2/3/4 (tȁ is a superessive postposition, ‘on, upon’), but ‘5’ has contracted into one featural foot with the postposition in all the forms. For numerals above 10, the model is consistently decimal. The word for tens, kɔ̏ŋ̏ ~ kɒ̏ɒ̏ŋ̏, is etymologically related to the noun kɔ̏ ‘hand / two hands’. The word for ‘hundred’, kʌ̄ŋ̄, is a noun, most probably a Manding loan (cf. above for the discussion concerning the word kɛ̀mɛ in Bamana). The word gblɯ̋ ‘1000’ is also a noun; its original meaning is ‘basket’ (for cola nuts),17 cf. above on the etymology of the word wáa ‘1000’ used for money counting in Bamana.

95Addition manifests itself through several models:

  • for the numerals 6 through 9, the model is reconstructible as *5 tȁ X, where X = 1 through 4 (see above);
  • for numerals above 10 and indivisible by 10, the model is: “Nx10 ɤ̄ gā N”; ɤ̄ gā means literally ‘its bone’ (ɤ̄ is a singular reflexive pronoun, while gā is a polysemous noun whose basic meaning is ‘bone’ or ‘grain’, but also ‘unit’, cf. (Erman 2005));
  • for numerals above 100, the previous model (construction with the singular reflexive pronoun) competes with the regular coordinative construction expressed by the coordinative personal pronoun wāȁ (on coordinative pronouns in Eastern Dan, see (Vydrin 2010)); for more detail see (Vydrin 2017: 487–488). In the construction with the reflexive pronoun, the word for ‘10’ is kɔ̏ ‘hand / pair of hands’, rather than kɒ̏ɒ̏ŋ̏ (the latter is certainly derived from kɔ̏, although it is unclear what element it has fused with).

96Multiplication is expressed through simple juxtaposition of the multiplicand and the multiplier. The multiplicands in Dan-Gwɛɛtaa are kɒ̏ɒ̏ŋ̏ ‘10’, kʌ̄ŋ̄ ‘100’, gblɯ̋ ‘1000’, gblɯ̋ ɓɛ̰̄ɛ̰̏ ‘1,000,000’.

97Subtraction and division are absent from the Dan-Gwɛɛtaa numeral system.

98External syntax. The basic syntax of the numerative construction is the same as elsewhere in the Mande family: the numeral follows the noun, and, most often, the plural marker ɗṵ̏ is absent: sààgā sɔ̋ɔ̋ɗű ‘5 arrows’. However, this marker can appear after the noun or after the numeral, and in both cases it expresses restrictive meaning: ɓɛ̰̄ sɔ̋ɔ̋ɗű-ɗṵ̋ = ɓɛ̰̄-ɗṵ̏ sɔ̋ɔ̋ɗű ‘exactly 5 persons’.

99In complex numerals, the count noun is repeated after each component (9a), with the exception of those cases where the reflexive pronoun is used as connector (9b)

(9a) tɔ̏ kʌ̄ŋ̄ dō wāȁ tɔ̏ kɒ̏ɒ̏ŋ̏ dō wāȁ tɔ̏ dō
chicken 100 1 they.and chicken 10 1 they.and chicken 1
or
(9b) tɔ̏ kʌ̄ŋ̄ dō ɤ̄ kɔ̏ dō ɤ̄ gā dō
chicken 100 1 refl.sg hand one refl.sg bone one
‘111 chickens’

100There are several counting words in Dan-Gwɛɛtaa which diverge in their degrees of grammaticalization (all can also be used as regular nouns). The most advanced in this direction (although still not quite obligatory in their use) are:

  • gā ‘single object out of a mass/group’ (original meaning: ‘bone, grain, drop’). This word has further grammaticalized as a connector in compound numerals, see above;
  • ɓɛ̄ ‘oblong object’ (original meaning: ‘fruit’);
  • ɗʌ̰́ ‘living being’ (original meaning: ‘child’).

101Even less grammaticalized in the function of classifiers or measure words are the following nouns: kpɤ̄ ‘ball’ (for spherical objects), gèè ‘(dead) body’ (for carcasses of animals), bɤ̋ɤ̋ ‘bunch, bundle’ (for carcasses of animals), ya̰̋ ‘eye’ (for the content of receptacles).

2.6. San-Maka numerals

102San-Maka (Eastern Mande < Southeastern Mande) is spoken by some 230,000 people in the province of Nayala in Burkina Faso (Berthelette 2001). The data were collected by Elena Perekhvalskaya during her fieldwork; additional data are drawn from (Paré 1999). The tilde ~ separates off phonetic variants.

Table 8. San-Maka numerals

1 gōōŋ̀
2 páā
3 sɔ̄ɔ̄
4 síí
5 sórō
6 sɔ̄rɔ̄
7 sɔ̄bāā
8 kíwísí
9 mānāgɔ̀rɔ̄
10 fù
11 fù lù gōōŋ̀ (10+1)
12 fù lù páā (10+2)
13 fù lù sɔ̄ɔ̄ (10+3)
20 fòó
21 fòó ŋ́ gōōŋ̀ (20+1)
25 fòó ŋ́ sórō (20+5)
30 fù-sɔ̄ɔ̄ ~ pù-sɔ̄ɔ̄ (10x3)
33 fù-sɔ̄ɔ̄ ká sɔ̄ɔ̄ (10x3+3)
35 fù-sɔ̄ɔ̄ ká sórō (10x3 + 5)
37 fù-sɔ̄ɔ̄ ká sɔ̄bāā (10x3 + 7 (5+2))
40 fù-síí ~ pù-síí (10x4)
50 fù-sórō ~ pò-sórō (10x5)
57 fù-sórō ká sɔ̄bāā (10x5 + 7 (5+2))
60 fù-sɔ̄rɔ̄ ~ pɔ̀ sɔ̄ɔ̄ (10x6)
70 fù-bɔ̄-mā-lɛ́ (-10)
80 pāābīɛ̄ ~ bīɛ̄
90 pāābīɛ̄ ká fù (80+10)
100 pāābīɛ̄ ká fòó, bīɛ̄ fòó (80+20)
150 pāābīɛ̄ ká fù-bɔ̄-mā-lɛ́, bīɛ̄ fù-bɔ̄-mā-lɛ́ (80+70 (-10))
151 pāābīɛ̄ ká fù-bɔ̄-mā-lɛ́, bīɛ̄ fù-bɔ̄-mā-lɛ́ ká gōōŋ̀ (80+70 (-10)+1)
160 bīɛ̄-páā (80x2)
800 dúú gōōŋ̀ (800x1)
64,000 dúú-pāābīɛ̄ (800x80)
576,000 dúú-bīɛ̄ mānāgɔ̀rɔ̄ (800x720 (80x9))
639,279 dúú-bīɛ̄-mānāgɔ̀rɔ̄ ká fùbɔ̄mālɛ́ ká mānāgɔ̀rɔ̄ (800x799 {720 (80x9) + 79 [(80-10) + 9]}).
1,000,000 mílíɲɔ́ɔ́nɛ́ góōŋ̀
1,000,000,000 mílíáárè góōŋ̀
  • 18 It is true that in San-Maka, ‘3’, sɔ̄ɔ̄, may look like a better candidate, however, this similarit (...)

103A quinary system in San-Maka is no longer evident, but traces of it can be found. Interpretation of the term for 7 sɔ̄bāā as 5+2 is rather straightforward: the element -bāā goes back to páā ‘two’ (consonant voicing in word-internal position is common in Mande languages, and the tonal modification HM > MM can be explained by the phonetic integration of the components of the former compound), and the initial element, sɔ̄- goes back to ‘5’, sórō18. Sɔ̄rɔ̄ ‘6’ is most likely a compound ‘5+1’ by origin, where the initial element sɔ̄- is the same as in ‘7’, and the final –rɔ̄ goes back to Proto-Mande *dọ́. Therefore, the form gōōŋ̀ ‘1’ in San-Maka should be regarded as an innovation, i.e., the situation is very similar to that of wɔ́ɔrɔ ‘6’ in Manding; cf. a similar suggestion (although presented without details) in (Pozdniakov 2018: 219).

104The form for ‘8’, kíwísí (in other San varieties cígísí, kígísí, tīgīsī (Berthelette 2001: 62)) is opaque; it is certainly neither ‘5+3’, nor ‘10-2’. The only plausible interpretation along these lines would be ‘4x2’ (cf. síí ‘4’), otherwise it should be regarded as a non-derived word of unknown origin.

105The numeral for ‘9’, mānāgɔ̀rɔ̄, is most probably ‘10 – 1’ in origin, in which case the element -gɔ̀rɔ̄ can be interpreted as an archaic form of ‘1’, and the initial element mānā- is opaque (at least at the current stage of our knowledge of San-Maka).

  • 19 Frequently used in compound numerals in an abridged form, bīɛ̄ ‘bundle’, in which case the subseq (...)

106There is a special (non-compound) word for ‘20’ which seems to be the only relic of a vigesimal system; it does not appear in numerals divisible by 20. To the contrary, two other base numerals, 80 and 800, are relevant. The numeral for 80 pāā bīɛ̄ ‘bundle of cowries’19 is the fundamental base of the octogesimal model. The term dúú ‘800’ is a noun, rather than a numeral; it is obligatorily accompanied by a numeral (dúú gōōŋ̀ ‘800’ = 800x1).

107The peculiarity of the San-Maka system is a complete lack of 100 and 1000 count bases.

108In addition, the following strategies are used.

109a) in the formation of the numerals 6 and 7, we can postulate addition without a connector (a fact obscured by fusion);

110b) with the linker lù, in the formation of the numerals 11-19: fù lù sɔ̄ɔ̄ ‘13’ (10+3);

111c) with the linker ŋ́ (a possessive marker), for the numerals 21-29: fòó ŋ́ sɔ̄ɔ̄ ‘23’ (20+3); fòó ŋ́ kíwísí ‘28’ (20+8);

112d) with the linker ká (a comitative marker) for the addition:

  • of units to tens in numerals bigger than 30: fù-sɔ̄ɔ̄ ká sórō ‘35’ (10x3 + 5); fù-sórō ká sɔ̄bāā ‘57’ (10x5 + 7 (5+2));
  • of the tens to 80: pāā-bīɛ̄ ká fù-bɔ̄-mā-lɛ́ ‘150’ (80+[80-10]).

113multiplication is expressed by juxtaposition. The multiplicands are 10, 80 and 800. The multipliers with 10 are limited to 3 through 6. With 80, multipliers 2-9 are used. With 800, the multiplier can be any number from 1 to 799: dúú fù-síí ‘32000’ (800x40); dúú-bīɛ̄-mānāgɔ̀rɔ̄ ká fùbɔ̄mālɛ́ ká mānāgɔ̀rɔ̄ ‘639200’ (800x799 {720 (80x9) + 79 [(80-10) + 9]}).

114Subtraction in San-Maka is used in the formation of the numeral ‘70’, fù-bɔ̄-mā-lɛ́ (lit. “ten take.away-me-on”) , i.e. ‘minus ten’: in this construction the numeral pāā bīɛ̄ ‘80’ is not named, but implied.

115Counting words. In San-Maka there is one counting word, māŋ́ ‘thing’, for all types of counted objects. It is used with every numeral except for gōōŋ̀ ‘one’. Moïse Paré considers this classifier to be optional (Paré 1999: 81), but in Elena Perekhvalskaya's field data it appears consistently, except in the context of nouns denoting people: nɛ̄lɔ́ páá ‘two daughters’, but cōŋ̄ màŋ́ páá ‘two houses’. The counting word also appear when numerals are used in predicative function:

(10) Māā cōŋ̄ nə̄ màŋ́ sɔ̄ɔ̄.
1sg-poss house ipf thing three
‘I have three houses (my houses are three)’.

116External syntax. Numerals follow the determined noun which appears without the plural marker ŋ̄/ -nə́, see examples above.

2.7. Soninke numerals

117Soninke is a Western Mande language spoken by more than 2 millions people in Mali, Senegal and Mauritania, but also in the Gambia and Burkina Faso. Soninke was the dominant language of the first great empire of Western Sudan, Ghana (or Wagadu), and exerted a tangible influence on the languages of the region. Soninke differs considerably from other Mande languages in many respects, this divergence is particularly visible in terms of the morphology and syntax of the numeral system. Our analysis of Soninke numerals is based primarily on (Creissels & Urmanchieva 2017: 261–262), but also on (Diagana 1995: 150–159) and (Diagana 1994: 249–274). When the sources provide different forms (which may be due to dialectal divergence), we give the forms from (Creissels & Urmanchieva 2017).

Table 9. Soninke numerals

1 báané
2 hílló
3 sìkkó
4 nàxàtó
5 kàràgó
6 tùnmú
7 ɲérú
8 ségú
9 kàbú
10 tánmú
11 tánmú dò báané
20 tánpíllé
30 tánjíkké
40 tánnáxáté
50 tánkárágé
60 tándúmé
70 tánɲéré
80 táncégé
90 tánkábé
100 kàmé
200 kàmó hìllì
1000 wújìné
6000 wújùnú tùnmì
1,000,000 mílíyò
  • 20 It cannot be excluded altogether that kàbú ‘9’ is a subtractive numeral in origin, in which case (...)

118The Soninke numeral system is strictly decimal. As in Dzuungoo (and in contrast to the majority of Mande languages), there are no traces of the quinary model even in the first ten: ‘6’ tùnmú and ‘7’ ɲérú seem to be simple stems, both having good cognates in Bozo languages (Bozo-Tigemaxo (Anonyme 1982): ‘6’ tuumi, ‘7’ jyeni) and in the Samogho group (Dzuungoo (Solomiac 2014): ‘6’ tsùnmɛ̄n, ‘7’ ɲɛ̀ɛ̀nún). ‘8’ ségú has good cognates in Bozo, but also in the Manding and Mokole groups. ‘9’ kàbú is a common innovation in the Soninke-Bozo group (cf. Bozo-Tigemaxo kaawi, in other Bozo varieties kafi, kapi) with unclear etymology.20

119Arithmetic operations in the Soninke numeral system are trivial. Addition is used to combine numerals of different orders. It is always marked by the standard coordinative/ comitative preposition/conjunction `dó. Multiplication is applied to the bases tánmú/tán- ‘10’, kàmé/kàmó ‘100’, wújìné/wújùnú ‘1000’ and mílíyò ‘1,000,000’; the forms tán-, kàmó, wújùnú appear in compounds formed through multiplication (kàmó and wújùnú are, in fact, regular plural forms of kàmé and wújùné). In compound numerals with the multiplicand tán ‘10’, most multipliers appear in modified forms: they have high tone (whatever the tone of the original form may be), and the initial consonant undergoes alternation (in Soninke there is a regular alternation of initial consonants after the nasal element: N + s nc, N + h np, etc. However, consonant alternations in the names of tens are sometimes in disagreement with the general rule.). Ex. (Creissels & Urmanchieva 2017: 262):

(11) wújùnú tùnmì dó kàmó hìllì dó tán-jíkké dò tùnmú
1000 6 and 100.pl 2 and 10-3 and 6
‘6236’
  • 21 Only the main features of the morphosyntax of Soninke numerals are analyzed, some peripheral ones (...)

120Morphology and external syntax.21 There are three different strategies for combining numerals with the nouns they determine:

  • báané ‘1’ behaves as an adjective: it follows the determined noun which appears in its non-autonomous form, and the numeral assumes HL tone: yíttè ‘tree’ + báané ‘1’ → yíttí báanè ‘one tree’;
  • numerals from 2 through 10 follow the determined noun, they assume a low tone, and their final vowel is replaced by –i. The determined noun appears in its plural form: yàxàrê ‘woman’ (sg.), yàxàrû ‘women’ (pl.), ɲérú ‘7’ → yàxàrú ɲèrì ‘seven women’;
  • numerals from 20 and above, very atypically for Mande languages, precede the determined noun which appears in its singular form and assumes a tonal contour typical of the head noun in a genitival construction (LH): yúgúxásè ‘elderly man’ (sg.), yúgúxásò ‘elderly men’ (pl.), tánpíllé ‘20’ → tánpíllé yùgùxàsé ‘twenty elderly men’. One could interpret such constructions as genitive, where the numeral appears as a dependent noun; in this case, the literal translation would be something like “a score of elderly men”. However, this interpretation is contradicted by the syntax of compound numerals which combines the first and the second strategies above: the part for the tens (hundreds, thousands...) precedes the noun (which appears in plural form, if not followed by ‘1’), and the part for the units follows it, while the preposition/conjunction `dó ‘and’ is placed between the higher numeral and the count noun.
(12) wújúnè dó kàmó sègì dó tóngú ɲèrì
1000 and 100.pl 8 and hoe.pl 7
‘1807 hoes’ (Diagana 1995: 157)

3. A general survey of numeral systems in Mande family

121Table 10 displays the main characteristics of the numeral systems of 30 Mande languages (including the 8 languages analyzed above whose names are in bold) representing all 11 groups of the family. The total number of languages in the Mande family is between 60 and 70 (the definitive figure depends on decisions on the status of closely related varieties), so, in our sample, about half of the Mande languages are represented. There are good reasons to think that the languages remaining outside our sample might not display characteristics radically different from those included in our survey; in other words, we consider our survey to be quite representative.

122Table 10 is followed by concise comments on any interesting points of the numeral systems of those languages not analyzed above.

General comments on Table 10.

123The column “Numerals ‘5+N’” is relevant only for languages with elements of quinarity (even if marginal and/or synchronically obscure). In this column, numerals formed through addition with the augend ‘5’ are presented.

  • 22 For example, in Mano ‘12’ is vṵ̀ dōó wɛ́lɛ́ pèèlɛ̄ (10 1 grain 2). Therefore, vṵ̀ cannot be (...)

124In the column “Multiplicands”, all the numerals able to appear as multiplicands are provided, and the column “Augends” lists all the simple numerals or stems22 which can function as augends. If a numeral can be used in both functions, it is presented in both columns (e.g., 10 tâŋ in Mandinka). If a multiplicand or an augend is a bound morpheme, it is given with a dash (e.g., 5 sűa̋- in Kla-Dan which appears in compound numerals sűa̋àdò ‘6’ = ‘5+1’, sűa̋ȁplɛ̏ ‘7’ = ‘5+2’, etc.).

  • 23 We take as the standard test the ability or inability of a lexeme to express its basic meaning wit (...)

125Multiplicands displaying nominal, rather than numeral, properties23 are followed by the label “Nom”, and those with numeral properties prevailing have the label “Num”. The absence of a label means that the required information is lacking. This distinction is not relevant for lexemes/stems (such as as bî ‘10’ in modern Bamana or gọ́ ‘10’ in Goo) which can have only ‘2’ or more as multiplier; such forms are provided with the label “Irr”.

126In the column “Connectors”, connective elements are provided, with their literal meanings or etymologies and comments on their spheres of use. By default, the connectors are used in addition constructions; in the exceptional case of a multiplicative connector, this fact is specially mentioned.

Table 10. Summary table

Language

Simple numerals (<10)

Numerals ‘5+N’

Multiplicands

Augends

Subtrahends & division

Connectors

Bamana (old system)

1 kélen
2 fìla
3 sàba
4 náani
5 dúuru
8 séegin
9 kɔ̀nɔntɔn

6 wɔ́ɔrɔ
7 wóronwìla

80 kɛ̀mɛ Num
800 bà Nom

5 wÓ-
10 tán Num
20 mùgan Num
40 dɛ̀bɛ Num
60 màninkɛ̀mɛ Num
80 kɛ̀mɛ Num
100 sìlamɛyakɛmɛ

Sbtr.: 1, 2, 10, 20
Division:
5 dlá
(5 = 10/2)

ni ‘and’

Bamana (modern system)

1 kélen
2 fìla
3 sàba
4 náani
5 dúuru
8 séegin
9 kɔ̀nɔntɔn

6 wɔ́ɔrɔ
7 wóronwìla

10 bî Irr
100 kɛ̀mɛ Num
1000 bà Nom
1000 wáa Nom (for money)
1000000 míliyɔn Nom

5 wÓ-
10 tán Num
20 mùgan Num
100 kɛ̀mɛ Num

ni ‘and’

Mandinka (Creissels & Sambou 2013)

1 kílîŋ
2 fùlá
3 sàbá
4 náaní
5 lúulu
8 sáyí
9 kònóntó

6 wóoró
7 wórówùlá

10 tâŋ Num
100 kèmé Num ~ Nom
1000 wúlî ~ wílî Nom
1000000 mílíyóŋ Nom

5 wó-
10 tâŋ Num
20 mùwáŋ Num
100 kèmé Num ~ Nom

níŋ ‘and’

Kakabe (Vydrina 2017)

1 kélen`
2 fìla
3 sàba
4 náani
5 lɔ́ɔlu
8 ságin
9 kɔ̀nɔntɔ`

6 wɔ́ɔrɔ
7 wɔ́rɔwila

10 bíi` Irr
100 kɛ̀mɛ Num
1000 wáa Nom
1000000 míliyɔn Nom

5 wɔ́-
10 tán` Num
20 mùgan Num
100 kɛ̀mɛ Num

nín ‘and’

Vai (Welmers 1976)

1 lɔ̀ndɔ́
2 fè'á
3 sàkpá
4 náánì
5 sóó'ù

6 sôŋ lɔ̀ndɔ́
7 sôŋ fè'á
8 sôŋ sàkpá
9 sôŋ náánì

20 mɔ̀ (bándé) (‘entire person’) Num
100 hɔ́ndɔ̂ Nom
1000 táúsùŋ Nom?

5 sôŋ Num
10 tâŋ Num
20 mɔ̀ (bándé) (‘entire person’) Num

ø (<20),
àkɔ́ ‘after it’ (> 20)

Jogo-Ton (Sapozhnikova 2017)

1 dīɛ̄
2 fálá
3 sègbā
4 nāānì
5 sōolò

6 māarɔ́
7 màalā
8 màsēgbá
9 mànānī

20 kēlē Irr
100 kēmē Num
1000 wúlú Nom

5 mà-
10 tán, táá Num
20 kēlēmò Num
100 kēmē Num


Division:
50 kēmē tárá
(50 = 100/2)

tɔ́ ‘and’

Jogo-Ligbi (Persson & Persson 1976; Persson & Persson 2007)

1 díɛ́n
2 fàlà
3 sigbá
4 náani
5 sóoló

6 mɔ̀ɔdó,
7 màúlà,
8 màsègbá,
9 màdáané

20 cɛ̌lè
100 cɛ̀mé
1000 wúlú

5 mà- ~ mɔ̀ɔ-
10 táàn
15 tígán
20 kèlɛ̀mɔ́ Num
30 túdó ~ tírró
100 cɛ̀mé


Division:
50 cìmé tàdá, kyémé tàrá
(50 = 100/2)

tɔ́ ‘and’

Jalkunan (Heath 2017)

1 dúlì
2 flā
3 sīgbō
4 nāānī
5 sóóló

6 mī-īlō
7 mà-álā
8 mà-sīgbō
9 má-nānì

20 jáān Irr
1000 wàʔà Nom, búúlì Nom

5 mi- ~ ma-
10 táá/tàà Num
20 jálámà Num

rá (11-19)
(N20+10)
tú (N10 + units, >20)

Susu (Shluinsky 2017; Touré 1994)

1 kérén
2 fìrín
3 sàxán
4 náání
5 súúlí

6 sénní (?)
7 sólófèrè
8 sólómásàxán
9 sólómánáaní

10 tòngó Irr
100 kɛ̀mɛ́ Num
1000 wúlù Nom

5 sóló-
10 fùú Num
20 mɔ̀xɔ́ɲɛ̀ń Num
100 kɛ̀mɛ́ Num

nùń ‘and’

Kpelle (Konoshenko 2017)

1 tááŋ́, tɔ̀nɔ ~ tànɔ́ŋ́
2 hwèèlɛ̌ ~ hwèèlɛ́
3 hààbǎ ~ hààbá
4 nááŋ́
5 lɔ́ɔ́lí

6 mɛ̀ídà
7 mɛ̀ìhwéélɛ̀
8 mɛ̀ìháábà
9 mɛ̀ìnááŋ́

10 pòǔ Num
100 ŋwúŋ̀ Nom
1000 wáá Nom

5 mɛ̀í Num
10 pòǔ Num

kɔ́ú ‘bone, grain, unit’ for Nx10+N, Nx100+Nx10
ø for Nx1000 + Nx100

Looma (Mishchenko 2017; Mishchenko p.c)

1 gílà(g)
2 félé(g)
3 sáwà(g)
4 náánì(g)
5 dɔ́ɔ́lù(g)

(6 dòzìtà)
7 dɔ́fèlà
8 dɔ́sáwà

10 pù(g) Irr
100 ṵ́(g) Nom
1000 wà(g) Nom
1000000 wùlù(g) Nom

5 dO-
10 pùgɔ̀ Num

9 tàwù(g)
(*‘1-10’)

mázú ‘on, upon’ for Nx10+N,
ø for all upper orders

Loko (Vydrin & Morozova 2017)

1 ngèrá(ŋ), ngìlá(ŋ)
2 fèlè(ŋ), fèè(ŋ)
3 cáwá(ŋ), sáwá(ŋ)
4 nááí(ŋ), náí(ŋ)

5 ndɔ́(ŋ), ndɔ́ú(ŋ)

6 ngɔ́hita(ŋ)
7 ngɔ̀fɛ̀là(ŋ)
8 ngɔ̀sákpá(ŋ)

10 pubu Irr
100 kɛmɛ Nom
1000 wul Nom

5 ngɔ-
10 pùù(ŋ) Num

9 ká-rábúú(ŋ), raabu(ŋ)
(*‘1-10’)

mah̃u ‘on, above’

Bozo-Tigemaxo (Blecke 1996; Blecke p.c.)

1 sana; kɔ ~ kuɔ̃ ~ kuɔ in compounds
2 fende ~ pende
3 sio ~ siyo
4 nara
5 kɔlɔ
6 tuumi
7 jeeni
8 seki
9 kiɛwi ~ kiawi

10 tã (20=10x2, 30=10x3) Num
100 kɛmɛ Num
1000 gulu Nom

10 tã Num
40 lɛwɛ Num
60 mɛnikɛmɛ
80 kiɛnima ~ bambarakiɛrima
100 silambekiɛrima

70 = 80-10 kiɛnima tã kẽ

yee ‘and’

Soninke (Creissels & Urmanchieva 2017)

1 báané
2 hílló
3 sìkkó
4 nàxàtó
5 kàràgó
6 tùnmú
7 ɲérú
8 ségú
9 kàbú

10 tán- Irr
100 kàmé Num
1000 wújìné Num

10 tánmú Num
100 kàmé Num
1000 wújìné Num

? 10 kàbú
(*‘1-10’)

`dó ‘and’

Dzuungoo (Solomiac 2014)

1 sōō, sɔ̄n
2 fíí
3 zhììgī
4 nààlén
5 nùn
6 tsùnmɛ̄n
7 ɲɛ̀ɛ̀nún
8 ɲáálòn
9 kyèèrón

80 cɛ̀ɛ̀n Nom

10 tsyéù/téēŋ Num
20 mòò/móò Num
40 dzyèè Num
60 mùéy Num
80 cààn Num
800 gúrú-sɔ̄n Num

140 = 80x2-20
cɛ̀ɛ̀nf́íímòòyáá

ø (11-30, 50, 100, 120)
kó ‘and’ (>30)

Seenku (Prost 1971)

1 sòèn
2 fi̋
3 sùɛ̏
4 nȁà
5 nò
6 tsȉìn
7 nyèèn
8 kȁà
9 kȍomèn

20 te̋gé Irr
200 bi̋ Irr
(2000 dan)

10 tő Num
20 fűe̋ Num

nè ‘with’ (20xN + 10),
wɛ́ɛ̏ (special postposition for numerals, 10xN+unit)

Jowulu (Carlson 1993; Prost 1958)

1 tḛ̏ḛ̏nȉ
2 fúúlì
3 byàì (Prost dyue)
4 píírèì
5 tá̰á̰
10 byḭ̏

6 tá̰á̰mànì (Prost tonte)

20 yàà Irr
1000 wáá Nom

5 tá̰á̰ (5+1=6)
10 bȕ Num
20 kȍnȉ Num

7 jɔ̰̀ɔ̰̀-pɔ́nȉ (-3)
8 fúl-pɔ́nȉ (-2)
9 tḛ̏ḛ̏-pɔ́nȉ (-1)

nȉ ‘plus’ (11-19)
fá ‘add’ (20xN + N)

Bobo (le Bris & Prost 1981)

1 tèlè, tàlá
2 pə̄lā
3 sàà
4 nàà
5 kòò

6 kònālā,
7 kòpə̄rā,
8 kòrōsòòn,
9 kòrōnòòn

20 kìē Irr
100 zɔ̀, zɔ̀ō Irr
1000 sānā

5 kò- ~ kòrō-
10 fūn Num
20 kiòrō Num
100 zɔ̀lō Num
1000 sànōn

ø for 11-19, 20xN + 10, 100xN + 10
nē nɔ̄mā ‘with child’ for the others

Beng (Paperno & Maloletnyaya 2017)

1 dō
2 plāŋ̄
3 ŋā̰ŋ̄
4 síéŋ́
5 sɔ́ŋ́

6 sɔ́dɔ̄
7 sɔ́plā
8 sɔ́wà
9 sīsí

10 bū Num
100 làà Nom
1000 kɛ́ŋ́ Nom

5 sɔ́-
10 bū Num

àsḭ́ŋ́ for units
nà̰ ... lō ‘and’ for upper orders

Gban (Fedotov 2017)

1 dó
2 fɛ̰̋ḭ̋
3 yȉȁ
4 zȉe̋
5 sűű

6 sɛ̋dò,
7 sɛ̋fɛ̰̋ḭ̋,
8 sɛ̋ȁȁ,
9 ti̋zȉe̋

10 vȕ Num
100 lű Nom
1000 wlű Nom
10,000 kpë Nom
1,000,000 mlḭ̀ɔ̰́ Nom

5 sɛ̋-
10 vȕ Num

ɛ̏kpȁȁ ‘it is on it’ or wli̋ ‘bone, fruit’ for units
ø for upper orders

Guro (Kuznetsova 2008; Kuznetsova & Kuznetsova 2017)

1 dụ̄
2 fíé
3 yāá, yāáká
4 zḭ̀ḛ́ ~ zḭ̀yḛ́, zḭ̀ɛ̰̀kɛ̰́
5 sólú

6 sụ̄ẹ̄dụ̄ ~ sụ̄ẹ̄lụ̄
7 sụ̄làyíé
8 sụ̄lāá
9 sụ̄làzḭ̀ḛ́

20 yọ̄ Nom
200 wúlù Nom
2000 bàà Nom

5 sụ̄-
10 vū Num

lâ ~ tâ ‘on, above’, for 6-9, 11-19
wọ́lẹ́ ‘bone’ for 20-29
é tâ lē ‘on its...’ after bàà or wúlù

Yaure (Kushnir 2017; Kushnir p.c.)

1 tụ̏
2 fííli̋ ~ fi̋li̋
3 yààgá
4 sḭ̀ḭ̀jḛ̀
5 sóólú

6 šẹ́ẹ́dụ̏
7 šálàvi̋li̋
8 šálàà
9 šálàsḭ̀ḛ̀

10 fȕ Num
100 yȁȁ Nom
1000 kpȉ Nom

5 šá-
10 fȕ Num
(20 yọ̏ Nom)

là ‘on’ for 7-9 (also 6?)
tá ‘on’ for 11-19
wẹ́lẹ́ ‘bone’ for 21-99
ø for ‘100xN + 10xN’ and for ‘1000xN + 100xN’
pá multiplier for 30-90

Mwan (Perekhvalskaya 2017)

1 dō
2 plɛ̄
3 yāgā
4 yīzīɛ̄
5 sóó

6 slúádō
7 slúáplɛ̄
8 slɔ́ā
9 slúáyīziɛ̄

20 mīā Nom
100 kɛ̀mɛ̀ Nom
1000 wáá Nom

5 slú-
10 vū Num

-á- for 5-9
tā ‘on’ for 11-19
ɓɛ̄ ‘grain’ for ‘20N+10’, ‘20N+N’
ké ‘with’ for ‘100xN + 10xN’
ø for higher orders

Dan-Gwɛɛtaa (Vydrin 2017)

1 dō
2 plɛ̀ ~ pȅȅdʌ̄
3 yȁȁgā
4 yȉȉsīɤ̄
5 sɔ̋ɔ̋ɗű

6 sɒ̋ɒ̏dō
7 sɒ̋ɒ̏plɛ̀
8 sa̋ȁgā
9 sűȅsīɤ̄

10 kɒ̏ɒ̏ŋ̏ Nom/ kɔ̏
100 kʌ̄ŋ̄ Nom
1000 gblɯ̋ Nom
1,000,000 gblɯ̋ ɓɛ̰̄ɛ̰̏ Nom

5 sO-

*tȁ ‘on’ for 6-9
ɤ̄ gā ‘its grain/unit’ for 11-99
wāȁ ‘and’ >100

Kla-Dan (Makeeva 2017)

1 dò
2 plɛ̏ ~ pȅȅdʌ̀
3 yȁȁgà
4 yȉȉsìè
5 sɔ̋ɔ̋lű

6 sűa̋àdò
7 sűa̋ȁplɛ̏
8 sűa̋ȁ
9 sűa̋ȁyȉȉsìè

10 kȕȁŋ̏/kɔ̏ŋ̏ Nom
100 kʌ̀ŋ̋ Nom
1000 wa̋a̋ Nom

5 sűa̋-

-ȁ- < *tȁ ‘on’ for 6-9

(à) gà ‘(its)grain/unit’ > 10

Mano (Khachaturyan 2017)

1 dōó
2 pèèlɛ̄
3 yààkā
4 yììsɛ̄
5 sɔ́ɔ́lī

6 sáládō
7 sálápèèlɛ̄
8 sálákā, sáláyììkā
9 sɛ́lɛ́sɛ̀, sɛ́lɛ̄ìsɛ̀

10 vṵ̀ Nom
100 ŋwū Nom
1000 wáá Nom

5 sá-

-lá- for 6-9
wɛ́lɛ́ ‘bone, grain’ for ‘10xN + 1xN’
là ‘on’ for ‘100xN + 10xN’, ‘1000xN + 100xN’

Tura (Idiatov 2005; Idiatov & Aplonova 2017)

1 dó
2 pȉȉɗɛ́
3 yȁká
4 yȉsɛ́
5 sʋ̋ɗʋ̋, sőőɗű

6 sa̋ȁdó,
7 sa̋ȁpȉȉɗɛ̋,
8 sa̋ȁká,
9 sɔ̋ìsɛ́

10 kúȁ Irr (< 100), kɔ̏ɔ̏ (> 100)
100 kȁ̰ḭ́ Nom
1000 wa̋a̋ Nom

5 sa̋ȁ-/sɔ̋-
10 bùù Num

nḭ̀ wɛ̋ɛ̋ ‘and grain’ for ‘10xN + 1xN’
nḭ̀ for upper orders

Goo (Aplonova & Vydrin 2017)

1 dọ́ / dọ́ọ́
2 pẹ̏ẹ̏lʌ́ / pẹ̏ẹ̏lʌ̏
3 yȁȁká / yȁȁkȁ
4 yȉȉsʌ́ / yȉȉsʌ̏
5 sőőlú / sőőlű

6 sa̋ládọ́ / sa̋ládọ̏
7 sa̋lápẹ̏ẹ̏lʌ́ / sa̋lápẹ̏ẹ̏lʌ̏
8 sa̋la̋ȁká / sa̋la̋ȁkȁ
9 sẹ̋ẹ̏sᴧ́

10 gọ́ Irr
10 kɔ̀ɔ̀ Nom (in compounds ‘100N+10N’)
100 kɛ̏ȉŋ́ Irr
1000 gbi̋li̋ Nom

5 sA̋-
(10 gọ̋ọ̏ Nom)
100 kɛ̏ȉŋ́lȉ Num

-lȁ- for 6-9
wɛ̋ɛ̋ ‘grain’ for ‘10xN+1xN’
wáȁ pɔ̰̀ ~ wàȁ pɔ̰́ɔ̰́ ‘and thing’ for ‘100xN+1xN’

Wan (Nikitina 2017; Nikitina ms.)

1 dō
2 pīlɔ̄ŋ
3 ã̊
4 si̊i̊yá
5 sɔ̊ɔ̊lů
6 wã́ã́ŋ
7 síāã̊
8 séŋyã̄

9 sɔ̄lásīyá

(3 ã̊)
(4 si̊i̊yá )
(5 sɔ̊ɔ̊lů)
20 mlīŋ Nom
100 cɛ̀mɛ̰̀, kɛ̀mɛ̰̀ Nom
1000 wågå

(4 si̊i̊yá)
(5 sɔ̊ɔ̊lů)
10 sɔ́ŋyɔ̊lů Num

tā ‘on’ for 11-19
ɔ̄ŋ ‘each other’ for ‘20xN+10’

Boko (Jones 1998)

1 dó
2 plá
3 ’ȁȁó̰
4 si̋i̋ɔ̰̋
5 sɔ̋ó

6 sɔ́òlɔ́
7 sóòplá
8 sɔ́ȁàȍ̰

20 bȁ Nom
200 ɔ̏áȁ Nom

10 kwȉ/kúɛ̏-
15 gɛ̰̏ó
25 bȁásɔ̋ó

1, 2, 5; up to 25

ø for ‘6-8’, ‘20xN+5’, ‘20xN+10’
ŋ̏ … ȍ ‘with’ for >15 (not divisible by 5)

San-Maka (Paré 1999)

1 gōōŋ̀
2 páā
3 sɔ̄ɔ̄
4 síí
5 sórō
8 kíwísí
9 mānāgɔ̀rɔ̄

6 sɔ̄rɔ̄
7 sɔ̄bāā

10 fù Num
80 pāābīɛ̄, bīɛ̄ Num
800 dúú Nom

5 sɔ̄-
10 fù Num
20 fòó Num
80 pāābīɛ̄ Num

70 (80-10)
fù-bɔ̄-mā-lɛ́

ø for ‘6, 7’
lù for ‘11-19’
ŋ́ for ‘21-29’
ká for ‘10xN+1xN’ (>30), ‘80xN + 10xN’

127Comments on individual languages (not analyzed specially in section 2).

3.1. Mandinka

128Unlike in other Manding varieties, in compound numerals from 30 to 90 divisible by 10, it is the regular form tâŋ that is used, rather than *bî.

129In a numerative construction (noun + numeral), a numeral can have the plural marker –lu, always preceded by the definite article –o, to express definiteness. The same meaning can be expressed by the article –o without the plural marker (Creissels & Sambou 2013: 221–22).

3.2. Vai

130The numeral system, as represented in (Welmers 1976: 67-73), is very transparent. The forms for ‘6–9’ are made by simple juxtaposition of numerals (although ‘5’ appears in an abridged form, sôŋ instead of sóó'ù). ‘20’ mɔ̀ bándé (lit. ‘entire person’) is a combination of two separate words, rather than one compound word, and in numerals divisible by 20, the multiplier is inserted between mɔ̀ and bándé, e.g.: ‘40’ mɔ̌ fè'á bándé, ‘80’ mɔ̌ náánì bándé.

131Beginning from 100, the system is decimal; the numerals for 100 and 1000 are English loans.

3.3. Jogo-Ton

132The autonomous form for ‘20’, kēlēmò, could be etymologised as a compound whose initial element, kēlē, might go back to the widespread Western Mande form *kélén ‘1’ (while in modern Jogo ‘1’ is dīɛ̄), and mò is a shortened form of the word mɔ̀gɔ̀ ~ mògò ‘human being’. I.e., kēlēmò could originally mean ‘one human being’, which would be very much in line with the general tendency in the Mande family, and the multiplicand form kēlē could be regarded as a result of subsequent truncation. There are however some weak points in this etymology: taking into account the regular syntax of numerals, one would expect to find the elements in the inverse order (along the lines of *mò-kēlē).

133As in Old Bamana, Jogo uses division: ‘50’ is designated as kēmē tárá, lit. ‘half (tárá) of a hundred’.

3.4. Jogo-Ligbi

134The numeral tígán ‘15’ probably contains an archaic stem *kan or *gan ‘foot, leg’ (cf. 2.2 for a discussion of the etymology of ‘15’ in Boko). However, in modern Ligbi the word for ‘foot, leg’ is kpɔ́, and the same form is attested in all the other languages of the group (Jogo, Jeri, Jalkuna). Therefore, if tígán is a relatively recent Jogo-Ligbi innovation, the element ‑gán is difficult to interpret as ‘foot’. According to (Persson & Persson 2007), there is a special word for ‘30’ (túdó or tírró), which may be an element of a heterogeneous quindecimal system (30 = 15x2).

135What was said about the etymology of ‘20’ in Jogo-Ton can also be applied to the Jogo-Ligbi forms kèlɛ̀mɔ́ / cɛ̌lè.

3.5. Jalkunan

136The tones borne by numerals undergo significant modifications depending on the context. Space constraints prevent us from presenting these modifications here; for the details see (Heath 2017: 90–95).

137The forms for ‘20’, jálámà / jáān, may go back to a proto-form similar to those in Jogo and Ligbi (*kélé-mɔ̀), although the sonorization of the initial consonant (*k- > *c- > j-) is yet to be explained. The irregular form for ‘20’ jén appearing in the numeral ‘30’, jén-tàà, may display the original vowel of the form ‘20’.

138One of the terms for ‘1000’, wàʔà, seems to be a Manding borrowing. The other, búúlì, also means ‘finger’ (Heath 2017: 94), and may be a remnant of the extended bodypart count.

139The connectors rá andare most probably allomorphs of one and the same morpheme (Heath, p.c.), which may be identical to the article-like noun suffix in Jalkunan which has the same forms.

3.6. Susu

140The form tòngó used as the base for the names of tens is homonymous with the verb tòngó ‘take’ (Touré 1994: 166), which can presumably be explained through particularities of Susu finger counting.

141The form for ‘20’, mɔ̀xɔ́ɲɛ̀ń, looks very much like the word for ‘human being’ in Manding and other languages of the Great Manding macro-group (Mokole, Vai-Kono, Jogo-Jeri); in Susu the corresponding word may be etymologically related too, but it has different vocalization: mìxí ‘person’. A borrowing from Manding or Mokole cannot be excluded.

3.7. Kpelle (Guinean variety)

142ŋwúŋ̀ ‘100’ probably goes back to ŋwùŋ̌ ‘head’, although the tonal difference between these forms is yet to be explained.

143The use of the connector kɔ́ú is not limited to units. For higher orders, addition is expressed by juxtaposition; optionally, the counted noun can be repeated before each order. When counting years, the postposition púlû ‘after, behind’ can be optionally inserted between the thousands and the hundreds (Konoshenko, p.c.):

(13)

gɔ̀láŋ

ŋwáá

tɔ́nɔ́

!púlû

ŋùŋ́

mɛ̀ìnááŋ́ 

kɔ́ú

pòú

mɛ̀ìnááŋ́

kɔ́ú

lɔ́ɔ́lí 

year

1000

1

after

100

9

grain

10

9

grain

5

the year 1995

3.8. Looma (Guinean variety)

144In the form dòzìtà ‘6’, dò- goes back to dɔ́ɔ́lù(g) ‘5’, while the element -zìtà may contain the archaic stem for ‘1’, -ta, cf. tá ‘certain, some’ (cf. also Kpelle tááŋ́ ‘one’). The element -zì- may be etymologically related to the inessive postposition that appears in different Looma dialects in the forms sù/zù and sì/zì.

145Just as in Guinean Kpelle, ṵ́(g) ‘hundred’ is similar to ṵ̀(g) ‘head’; the Kpelle and the Looma forms are undoubtedly identical etymologically (Looma tones are regularly the inverse of the tones found in Kpelle). In all likelihood, this word for ‘100’ can be explained as reflecting an extended body-part system (unfortunately, we have no information on the ways in which body parts are used for counting among different Mande peoples).

146In Guinean Looma, both stems for ‘1000’ found widely across the Western Mande languages, wà(g) and wùlù(g), do appear; however, the latter has been reinterpreted as ‘1,000,000’.

147There is one (at least etymologically) subtractive numeral: 9 tàwù(g), where -wù(g) < pù(g) ‘10’, and the initial element tà- may go back to the word tá ‘certain, some’ (cf. Kpelle tááŋ́ ‘one’). If this etymology is correct, this form violates Greenberg's (1990: 278) 10th generalization: “Subtraction is never expressed by the mere sequence of the subtrahend and the minuend”.

3.9. Loko

148In the form ngɔ́hita(ŋ) ‘5’, the element –hita(ŋ) can be interpreted in the same way as –zìtà in Looma (Loko h : Looma z is a regular correspondence), while ngɔ́- is an archaic stem for ‘5’ used in compounds.

149There are special forms for two numerals used in counting: kárábúú(ŋ) ‘9’ and kɛ́pù(ŋ), kápùù ‘10’. The etymology of the element ká-/kɛ́- is not clear.

150The reduplicated form pubu that appears in compound numerals divisible by ten (pubu fee ‘20’, pubu naai ‘40’, etc.) is certainly the plural form of pùu(ŋ) ‘10’; in Loko reduplication is a regular means of plural formation.

151There is one numeral which is subtractive by origin, 9 rábúú(ŋ). It seems to be composed according to the same model as tàwù(g) ‘9’ in Looma.

3.10. Bozo-Tegemaxo

152Oddly, ‘20’ is tã fende ‘10x2’ and ‘30’ is tã sio ‘10x3’, while the other numbers divisible by 20 are designated by special words, and ‘50’ = ‘40+10’ (lɛwɛ ye tã), ‘90’ = ‘80+10’ (kiɛnima ye tã). This fragment of the counting system, most probably, is borrowed from Old Bamana (or, alternatively, one could speculate that ‘40’ was borrowed by Old Bamana from Bozo). There is only one subtractive numeral, 70 = 80 – 10 kiɛnima tã kẽ lit. “80 ten lack”.

3.11. Seenku

153The word for ‘20’ used as an augend, fűe̋, could be a loan from the Eastern Mande languages, cf. San-Maka fòó. The origin of the multiplicand for ‘20’, te̋gé, is not quite clear; an ancient borrowing from Senufo or Teen (Gur family) cannot be excluded (cf. footnote 10). ‘200’ is expressed by a compound te̋gé tő ‘20x10’, and the base bi̋ ‘200’ is used only in multiplication, beginning from 400 (bi̋ fi̋ ‘200x2). dan ‘2000’ looks like a numeral base; however, it is not used as such, because for higher numeral Seenku speakers usually switch into Jula.

154The numerals follow the determined nouns, which appear in their plural form (with the exception of those preceded by the numeral ‘1’). In compound numerals, the determined noun is repeated before the component belonging to each order.

3.12. Jowulu

155Quinary counting is represented only by the form tá̰á̰mànì ‘6’ where the initial element is certainly tá̰á̰ ‘5’, but the final element is unclear. In (Prost 1958) we find the form tonte ‘6’ whose final element seems to be more transparent and can be traced back to the numeral ‘one’, tḛ̏ḛ̏nȉ (Carlson 1993), têna (Prost 1958).

156One would expect a numeral base ‘200’, but in (Carlson 1993) numerals between 100 and 1000 are missing. The form yàà ‘20’ used in compounds (‘40’, ‘60’, ‘100’, etc.) means ‘rope’ (evidently it goes back to the name for a bunch of cowrie shells). The subtractive numerals are jɔ̰̀ɔ̰̀-pɔ́nȉ ‘7’, fúl-pɔ́nȉ ‘8’ and tḛ̏ḛ̏-pɔ́nȉ ‘9’, see discussion of these forms in (Pozdniakov 2018: 224).

3.13. Bobo

  • 24 Le Bris et Prost (1981: 39) mention that the root zɔ̀lō, pl. zɔ̀ō is “proper to South Mande lang (...)

157Despite the existence of a numeral base for 100 zɔ̀lō24 whose abridged variant also appears in higher numerals divisible by 100 (200 zɔ̀ pə̄lā, 300 zɔ̀ō sàà, 400 zɔ̀ō nàà, etc.), numerals above 100 are formed according to the vigesimal principle: 120 kìē kònālā (20x6), 180 kìē kòrōnòòn (20x9), etc.

158The numeral ‘1’ is tèlè when counting, and tàlá when it determines a noun.

3.14. Beng

159In the numerals 6 through 9, compounding is accompanied by a loss of nasalization, cf.

  • ‘2’ plāŋ̄ vs. ‘7’ sɔ́plā,
  • ‘3’ ŋā̰ŋ̄ vs. ‘8’ sɔ́wā,
  • ‘4’ síéŋ́ vs. ‘9’ sīsí,25
  • and also ‘5’ sɔ́ŋ́ vs. ‘6’ sɔ́dō.

160However, an alternative interpretation is also possible: in most other South Mande languages, numerals 2 through 5 have no nasal elements. It is probable that the Beng numerals for 6 through 9 have retained the original forms of the root, while numerals 2 through 5 have undergone nasalization.

161Besides sɔ́wà (which is used as a simple form), there is an alternative form for ‘8’, kēŋēsíéŋ́, which appears in compound numerals (e.g. bū kēŋēsíéŋ́ ‘80’, kēŋēsíéŋ́-kēŋēsíéŋ́ ‘eight by eight, in eights’, a distributive meaning). Evidently, the final element of this form, síéŋ́, stems from síéŋ́ ‘4’, and kēŋēsíéŋ́ might be a multiplicative form, 4x2. However, the etymology of the initial element, kēŋē-, remains obscure.

162The form èbū ‘10’ appears in counting, otherwise, the shorter form bū is used.

163Numerals for the second and the third tens display an optional tendency towards fusion: būwlāŋ̄ ~ būplāŋ ‘20’, bṵ̄ā̰ŋ̄ ~ būŋā̰ŋ ‘30’.

164An alternative form for ‘1000’ is attested, là bū (100x10); it is used in the “forest dialect” of Beng.

3.15. Gban

165The numerals 6 through 8 have an initial element sɛ̋- which probably results from a merger of the numeral ‘5’ sűű with an undetermined locative postposition. The irregular consonant t- in the numeral ti̋zȉe̋ ‘9’ can probably be explained through regressive consonantal dissimilation (accompanied by a regressive assimilation of the vowel): *sɛ̋-zȉe̋ > ti̋zȉe̋.

166In complex numerals ending in ‘10’, the numeral ‘10’ appears in the “heavy” form vȕcɛ̰̀, e.g.: lű dò vȕcɛ̰̀ ‘110’. The meaning and etymology of the element cɛ̰̀ is unclear (it is perhaps a multiplier ‘1’).

167According to Maxim Fedotov (2017: 917), the word for ‘100’ may go back to lű ‘mouth’; if so, this may be another manifestation of extended bodypart counting.

3.16. Guro

168The numerals 3 and 4 have “heavy” forms, yāáká ~ yāāká and zḭ̀ɛ̰̀kɛ̰́, used in isolation or in the final position of compound numerals, which seem to express an emotional evaluation of the quantity: “as many as 3 (4)” (Kuznetsova 2008: 413). Comparison with other Southern and Eastern Mande languages suggests that the “heavy” form for ‘3’, yāáká, may be the original one (in which case yāá should be regarded as a shortened form), but for zḭ̀ɛ̰̀kɛ̰́ ‘4’ this is less probable (cf. however the Eastern Mande forms from the Boko-Busa cluster: Boko si̋i̋ɔ̰́, Bokobaru síígɔ̃).

169Wúlù ‘200’ seems to be a Manding loan. The semantic shift for wúlù (1000 > 200) may be explained either in terms of the discrepancy between the Jula decimal model and the Guro vigesimal model (i.e., its status as a numeral base has been maintained, but with a change to its numerical value), or through a confusion between money count (where the basic unit is 5 Fr) and object count; cf. (Zepp 1983: 149–150) on similar cases of confusion between 200 and 1000 in different languages of Côte d’Ivoire. Bàà ‘2000’ is homonymous with the word for a basket made of palm leaves (supposedly used in earlier times for carrying cola nuts). Yɔ̄ ‘20’ might go back to yɔ̄ ‘oil palm’, although it is not quite clear which components of the oil palm were counted.

170Noun precedes numeral. In compound numerals, the noun can be repeated before each order (in which case the connective element é tâ lē can be omitted), although it is rarely repeated before the number for the lowest order (units).

171The plural marker lṵ̂ is most often absent from the numerative, however, it can be present following either the noun or the numeral. It marks focus.

3.17. Yaure

172This language is the closest relative of Guro, so the forms and meanings of the numerals up to 20 are very similar to those found in Guro. The divergence concerns the numeral system after 20: it is consistently vigesimal in Guro and consequently decimal in Yaure. The word for ‘1000’ kpȉ (in other sources, kpȉȉ) is of unknown origin (cf. however kpë ‘10,000’ in Gban which could be cognate with kpȉ ).

173Yaure seems to be the only Mande language to make use of a connector (pá, most probably from the verb pá ‘take’) marking multiplication for tens: fȕ pá yààgá ‘30’, fȕ pá šálàsḭ̀ḛ̀ ‘90’. In the higher orders, multiplication is expressed by simple juxtaposition: yȁȁ fííli̋ ‘200’, kpȉ fííli̋ ‘2000’.

174The plural marker lṵ̀ can appear after the noun or after the numeral; it may also be absent.

3.18. Kla-Dan

175The Kla-Dan system is very close to that of Dan-Gwɛɛtaa, there is only one tangible divergence which concerns the word for 1000, wa̋a̋, borrowed in Kla-Dan from Manding. At the same time, the word gblɯ̋ (whose cognate yielded the term for ‘1000’ in Dan-Gwɛɛtaa) exists in Kla-Dan with the meaning of ‘temporary bag made of a palm leaf’.

3.19. Mano

176In the numerals ‘6-8’, the a-vocalization of initial syllables indicates the presence of a postposition (most probably, là ‘on’) as a connector between two simple numeral stems. In ‘9’, the change *a > ɛ is due to regressive assimilation.

  • 26 The formal similarity of the words for ‘head’ in Mano, a South Mande language, and Kpelle-Looma, S (...)

177The numeral ŋwū ‘100’ is homonymous with the noun for ‘head’ and may stem from an extended body-part counting system. Cf. above for Looma and Kpelle where words for ‘100’ and ‘head’ are quasi-homonymous and differ tonally.26

3.20. Tura

178There are three forms for ‘10’: bùù is used as an augend in the numerals from 10 through 19; kúȁ is a multiplicand ‘10’ in the numerals from 20 through 90; kɔ̀ɔ̀ is a multiplicand ‘10’ in compound numerals above 100. Both kúȁ and kɔ̀ɔ̀ go back, undoubtedly, to the noun kɔ̀ɔ̀ ‘hand, pair of hands’. The compound numeral for ‘100’ is kȁ̰ḭ́ dḭ̏nḭ̏ instead of the expected kà̰ḭ́ dó. Dḭ̏nḭ̏ is, to use Idiatov's (2005: 54) term, a “specified noun” meaning generally ‘the other (of two)’.

3.21. Goo

179Numerals 1 to 8 have different forms when used in the attributive construction (in Table 10 they are given before the slash), and in counting (after the slash). ‘10’ also has two forms: gọ̋ọ̏dọ́ in the attributive construction, gọ̋ọ̏dọ̏ in counting (this fact can be regarded as an argument for an interpretation of these forms as monolexemic in synchrony, rather than as a combination of two lexemes ‘10’ and ‘1’).

  • 27 Related forms are attested in Dan-Blo (a western Ivoirian dialect of Dan) where ‘10’ in compound n (...)

180As in Tura, here we observe two different stems for ‘10’ in the numerals divisible by ten: those which do not exceed 100 (10 gọ̋ọ̏dọ́, 30 gọ́ yȁȁká, etc.), and those which exceed 100 (110 kɛ̏ȉŋ́lȉ kɔ̀ɔ̀ dọ́, etc.). This distribution between two stems for ‘10’ might be more complex (and is yet to be verified in more detail); cf. example (14) where the form gọ́ appears in a context where one would expect kɔ̀ɔ̀. Gọ̋ọ́/gọ́ is probably etymologically related to gọ̋ọ́ ‘cola nut’27 (one could speculate that it originally designated a small package of cola), and kɔ̀ɔ̀ goes back to the noun kɔ̀ɔ̀ ‘hand, pair of hands’.

181One would expect the word for ‘1000’ gbi̋li̋ to stem from the noun for ‘cola basket’ (as in Dan-Gweetaa), however the name for ‘cola basket’ in Goo has a different form, gblì ~ gbìì (although its lexical tone is yet to be verified). One cannot exclude the possibility that gbi̋li̋ ‘1000’ in Goo has been borrowed from Dan.

182In compound numerals, the counted noun is repeated before each order, except for the units, where the numerative word wɛ̋ɛ̋ ‘bone, grain’ appears instead:

(14) kwẹ̀ẹ̀ gbi̋li̋ dọ́ kwẹ̀ẹ̀ kɛ̏ŋ́ sẹ̋ẹ̏sᴧ́ kwẹ̀ẹ̀ gọ̋ sẹ̋ẹ̏sᴧ́ wɛ̋ɛ̋ sa̋ládọ́
year 1000 1 year 100 9 year 10 9 bone 6
‘the year 1996’

3.22. Wan

  • 28 The “upper ring” symbol is used by Tatiana Nikitina for the “neutral high” tone, an underlying ton (...)

183Forms for the numerals ‘6–10’ display strategies which are very unusual for Mande languages. Only sɔ̄lásīyá ‘9’ is formed according to the pan-Mande quinary model “5+4” (sɔ̊ɔ̊lů28 ‘5’ + si̊i̊yá ‘4’, with a connector postposition in-between, probably *tā ‘on’).

184The pair numerals (6, 8, 10) seem to be archaic plurals (or duals) formed through suffixation or infixation of a nasal syllabic (probably accompanied by an undetermined vowel; in any case, this derivation is accompanied by irregular modifications of the stems):

  • ‘3’ ã̊ — ‘6’ wã́ã́ŋ,
  • ‘4’ si̊i̊yá — ‘8’ séŋyã̄,
  • ‘5’ sɔ̊ɔ̊lů — ‘10’ sɔ́ŋyɔ̊lů.

185Infixation is an extremely untypical procedure in Mande, although transfixation as a marker of superintensive form of adjectives is attested in Dan-Gwɛɛtaa (Vydrine 2007:84ff). If this infixation were present in only one pair of numerals (especially in the pair ‘3 — 6’ where infixation is not evident, because of the monosyllabic character of the numeral ‘3’), it would be appropriate to discard it as a fortuitous resemblance; however, its appearance in three pairs of numerals makes the assumption of a fortuitous resemblance rather improbable.

186Finally, síāã̊ ‘7’ looks like a combination of ‘4’, si̊i̊yá, and ‘3’, ã̊. The model “7 = 4+3” is not only exceptional in Mande, but is also rare in Niger-Congo as a whole (Pozdniakov 2018: 282).

187To sum up, the counting system of Wan for the second five looks (etymologically) as follows:

  • 6=3x2; 7=4+3; 8=4x2; 9=5+4; 10=5x2.

188The even numerals are formed through multiplication by two, and the odd numerals through addition of the largest possible items. Although very unusual for the Mande family, this system is not exceptional among the languages of the world. In fact, it is close to Type II of Seidenberg's “Neo-2” system (Hurford 1987: 250–251; Hanke 2010: 71–72), with the sole difference that the even numerals are derived by multiplication by two (expressed though infixation), rather than reduplication.

189The term for ‘20’, mlīŋ, can be compared with mlītī ‘people’ ((Nikitina ms.); cf. mī ‘human being’); as in many other Mande languages, it may stem from the noun for ‘human being’.

4. Major characteristic features of Mande numeral systems

4.1. Classification of numerical systems into types

190If we proceed from the principle formulated in 1.3 (”the multiplicand of the lower order, i.e., the radix, defines the type of the numerical system”), the following numerical systems exist in the Mande family:

  • decimal (Modern Bamana, Mandinka, Kakabe, Susu, Kpelle, Soninke, Beng, Gban, Yaure, Dan-Gwɛɛtaa, Kla-Dan, Mano, Tura, Goo),
  • pure vigesimal (Seenku, Jalkunan, Jowulu, Guro, Boko),
  • mixed decimal and vigesimal (Vai, Jogo, Ligbi, Bozo-Tigemaxo, Bobo, Mwan, Wan),
  • octogesimal (Old Bamana, Dzuungoo),
  • mixed decimal and octogesimal (San-Maka).

191By “mixed decimal and vigesimal”, we mean systems where “the numbers up to 99 are expressed vigesimally, but the system then shifts to being decimal for the expression of the hundreds, so that one ends up with expressions of the type x100 + y20 + z” (Comrie 2005). In the mixed decimal and octogesimal system of San-Maka, counting is decimal up to 79, and then switches to octogesimal (x80 + y10 + z).

192The Bozo-Tigemaxo system is peculiar: it is decimal up to 39 and from 100 on, but it is “heteroradical vigesimal” in the intermediate span (x + 10 + y, where x is divisible by 20). The same “heteroradical vigesimal” component is also present in both octogesimal systems (Old Bamana, Dzuungoo).

193Vigesimal models (pure or mixed with decimal ones) do not predominate in Mande, but they are well represented; they are characteristic of more than a third of the languages in our sample. It is interesting that vigesimal models are dispersed over diverse genetic groups and are not concentrated in one or two closely related ones. At the same time, nearly all Mande languages where vigesimal systems are attested (with the exception of Vai) are in close contact with Kru, Gur and/or Senufo languages (or such contact can be supposed at an earlier stage). This serves a crucial point in favor of the argument that the vigesimal model in Mande is contact-induced.

  • 29 The areal character of this phenomenon is mentioned, in particular, in (Hammarström 2010: 32).

194The octogesimal models represented in the Old Bamana system, in Dzuungoo and in San-Maka seem to be an areal innovation, perhaps resulting from the evolution of a vigesimal system during the development of inter-African commerce (the word for ‘80’ may be traced back either to the term for a package of cola, or to a string of cowrie shells). On the other hand, the substratum influence of the Senufo languages (where octogesimal systems are also present) is highly probable. 29

195In two languages, Jogo-Ligbi and Boko, elements of a pentadecimal system (built on base 15) have been identified: the word for 15 is not compound (at least in synchrony), it appears as an augend for the numerals from 16 through 19, and in Boko it also acts as a diminuend for 14. Pentadecimal systems are very rare in the languages of the world.

4.2. Elements of the quinary model and other models within the first ten

196No Mande numerical system is quinary in the strictest sense, given that a numeral for ‘5’ does not serve as a multiplicand in any language. However, it is very frequent as an augend, and there are reasons to think that this situation can be reconstructed at the Proto-Mande level. Quinary models never extended beyond the first ten, and in many languages the quinarity eventually faded away altogether: original components have often fused and lost their distinctiveness, and some numerals from 6 through 9 have lost their quinary nature. Remnants of this model are found in 25 out of 30 systems of our sample, and are missing only in Dzuungoo, Seenku, Bozo-Tegemaxo and Soninke (Wan, with its idiosyncratic model for the numerals of the second five, stands apart).

197Exceptional in Mande are the remnants of models with multiplicand 4: the form for ‘8’ used in compound numerals in Beng, kēŋēsíéŋ́, presumably ‘4x2’ by origin, see 3.14; ‘8’ in San-Maka, kíwísí, probably also ‘4x2’ by origin, see 2.6; the Wan form for ‘8’, and also for ‘6’ and ‘10’, see 3.22. However, these remnants of “Neo-2” structures are not so rare in Niger-Congo; in particular, this model (mainly for 4x2 = 8) is attested in some Gur languages (Natioro, Wara, Viemo, some Grusi languages), in numerous Adamawa languages, in Ijo, in the Ka-Togo and Na-Togo groups of the Kwa family, etc. (Pozdniakov 2018: 282–288), in Ekoi (Bantoid) (Hanke 2010: 71).

198Beng and Wan are Southern Mande languages which stand apart from the rest of the group; according to glottochronological analysis, their separation from the Proto-Southern Mande languages took place some 23-25 centuries ago, and the separation of San from its closest relative Bisa lies within the same temporal range (Vydrin 2009). The presence of non-quinary models in these languages might be interpreted as evidence for the lack of stability of count systems, even within the first ten, at that temporal distance.

4.3. Different forms for bases and corresponding numerals

199A phenomenon that often disguises the presence of quinarity (both for speakers and for researchers) is a mismatch between the forms for the numeral ‘5’ and the element referring to 5 in the numerals 6 through 9. In many languages (Vai, Susu, Looma, Bobo, Beng, Gban, Guro, Yaure, Mwan, Dan-Gwɛɛtaa, Kla-Dan, Mano, Tura, Goo, Wan, Boko, San-Maka) such formal differences result from assimilative processes accompanying the fusion of the elements and do not represent a great problem. There are however cases where we are undoubtedly dealing with different stems, see Table 11.

Table 11. Different stems for ‘5’ as a numeral and as an augend

  Bamana Mandinka Kakabe Jogo-Ton Jogo-Ligbi Jalkunan Loko Kpelle
5 dúuru lúulu lɔ́ɔlu sōolò sóoló sóóló ndɔ́(ŋ) lɔ́ɔ́lí
5 in compounds wóron-, wɔ́ɔ- wóro-, wóo- wɔ́rɔ-, wɔ́ɔ- māā-, màà- mɔ̀ɔ-, màú-, mà- ma-, mi- ngɔ- mɛ̀í-, mɛ̀ì-
  • 30 The element -rO- in Manding (and in Kakabe) probably goes back to the locative postposition *dɔ́ ( (...)

200Stems for ‘5’ in compounds are of two types: GO-/WO-30 (Bamana, Mandinka, Kakabe, Loko) and MA- (Jogo, Ligbi, Jalkunan, Kpelle).

201The stem MA- likely goes back to the superessive postposition broadly represented in the languages of the Mande family and reconstructible at the Proto-Mande level as *ɓã̀; the use of superessive adpositions as linkers in compound numerals is a well-attested phenomenon in Mande and elsewhere. If so, the original form of the numerals for 6-9 in Jogo, Ligbi, Jalkunan, Kpelle could be built on the model of “5 MA N”, with the subsequent truncation of the initial element.

  • 31 The Jowulu couple ‘10’ byḭ̏ bȕ is unclear in this respect. At the present state of knowledge a (...)

202Variability of forms with one and the same numerical value is also attested for higher numerals. Most often, a numeral has one form when it appears alone or as an augend, and another form when it functions as a multiplicand. Sometimes (Table 12), both forms are evidently variants of the same root (or, rather, one of the variants is derived from the other by affixation or compounding). There are however numerous cases (Table 13) where the stems belong to different roots.31

Table 12. Variability in stems formed from the same roots

Language 10 20 80 100
  sole or augend multipli­cand sole or augend multipli­cand sole or augend multipli­cand sole or augend multipli­cand
Jogo     kēlē kēlēmò        
Ligbi     kèlɛ̀mɔ́ cɛ̌lè        
Jalkunan     jálámà jáān, jén        
Looma pù(g) pùgɔ̀            
Loko pubu pùù(ŋ)            
Bozo-Tigemaxo             silambe­kiɛrima kɛmɛ
Soninke tánmú tán            
Dzuungoo         cààn cɛ̀ɛ̀n    
Bobo     kìē kiòrō     zɔ̀, zɔ̀ɔ̄ zɔ̀lō
Goo gọ̋ọ́ gọ́ < 100            

Table 13. Different stems in decimal and vigesimal systems

Language 10 20
  sole or augend multiplicand sole or augend multiplicand
Modern Bamana tán bî    
Kakabe tán bíi`    
Susu fùú tòngó    
Jowulu byḭ̏ bȕ kȍnȉ yàà
Seenku     fűe̋ te̋gé
Tura bùù kúȁ    
Goo gọ̋ọ́ kɔ̀ɔ̀ (>100)    

203With the exception of the Samogho languages (Jowulu and Seenku), for which our knowledge is very limited, the use of different roots for the same numerical values can be explained more or less satisfactorily. In Manding and Kakabe, BI-forms for ‘10’ most probably go back to the Proto-Mande root *pú, while tán may be a Western Mande innovation going back to the Proto-Niger-Congo stem for ‘5’ (Pozdniakov 2018: 224–226). In Susu, fùú is certainly an ancient form, and tòngó is a new one, probably resulting from the grammaticalization of the verb tòngó ‘take’. In Tura, bùù also represents the Proto-Mande root *pú, and kúȁ is an innovation of the Proto-Dan-Tura level going back to the word for ‘hand, arm; pair of hands’ (in Tura, kɔ̀ɔ̀); recall that in Dan, reflexes of *pú have been replaced by the form derived from “hand” (in Dan-Gwɛɛtaa, kɒ̏ɒ̏ŋ̏ or kɔ̏) in all contexts. In Goo, the “hand” form has replaced the original stem for ‘10’ in multiplicand function, but this form is used only in compound numerals above 100. In lower numerals another stem has emerged, which probably has the etymology ‘(small package of) cola nuts’.

4.4. Status of numerical bases

204Higher numerical bases tend to be more similar to nouns, and smaller bases behave more as true numerals (in the sense formulated in the general comments preceding Table 10). The only exception is the Goo language (Aplonova & Vydrin 2017: 461) where gọ̋ọ́ ‘10’ (used in the first hundred) and kɔ̀ɔ̀ ‘10’ (used in the second hundred and higher) are nouns (15, 16), while kḭ́ŋ́lḭ̏ ‘100’ is a numeral (16).

(15) kwi̋í gọ̋ọ́ dọ́ wɛ̋ɛ̋ pẹ̀ẹ̀lʌ́
basket 10 1 bone 2
‘12 baskets’ (10x1 + 2)
(16) kḭ́ŋ́lḭ̏ kɔ̀ɔ̀ dọ́
100 10 1
‘110’ (100 + 10x1)

205This unusual situation in Goo might be explained by the fact that both words for ‘10’ have etymologies more or less transparent for the speakers of this language (‘cola/package of cola’ and ‘pair of hands’), cf. 3.21, and kḭ́ŋ́lḭ̏ ‘100’ is most probably a Manding loan, and is etymologically opaque for speakers.

206Another peculiar case is represented by Boko where the numeral for ‘15’ probably contains the stem for ‘1’, cf. 2.2, and can thus be regarded as the lowest base displaying nominal characteristics.

  • 32 For certain languages of the sample, this information is unavailable or irrelevant; these language (...)

207According to the criterion “the lowest numerical base which has noun status”, the languages of our sample are distributed as follows:32

  • 10 — Dan-Gwɛɛtaa, Kla-Dan, Mano
  • 20 — Guro, Yaure, Mwan, Wan, Boko
  • 80 — Dzuungoo
  • 100 — Vai, Kpelle, Looma, Loko, Beng, Gban, Tura
  • 800 — Old Bamana, San-Maka
  • 1000 — Modern Bamana, Kakabe, Jogo, Jalkunan, Susu, Bozo-Tigemaxo, Jowulu
  • > 1000 — Soninke

208The situation in Mandinka is ambiguous (kème ‘100’ can behave as a numeral or as a noun). In Soninke, all bases, including wújìné ‘1000’, behave as numerals.

209A correlation emerges: languages of Sahelian communities with historical engagement in long-distance trade and state-building tend to transform counting bases into numerals to a higher degree. In Soninke, the language of the very first great empire of the Western Sudan, even ‘1000’ is a numeral. In Manding and Bozo (but also in other Sahelian languages), ‘100’ is a number, and only ‘1000’ is a noun. In languages spoken in the forest zone by communities which, until the most recent times, abstained from large-scale commerce, the “nominal threshold” is either ‘100’ (Southwestern and some Southern Mande languages) or ‘20’ (most of Southern Mande languages, Boko), or even ‘10’ (Dan, Mano). This correlation is highly natural: the more a language community is exposed to commerce, the more prevalent higher numeral bases are, because their frequent use facilitates their transformation into “true numerals”.

4.5. Subtraction and division

210Subtraction has been attested in nine languages, that is, in almost one third of our sample. However, in only two systems (Old Bamana, see 2.1, and Boko, see 2.2) is this operation productive. As mentioned in 2.2, some violations of the Greenbergian generalizations concerning subtraction are attested in the Boko system.

211In Looma and Loko, subtraction is used only for (etymologically opaque) ‘9 = 10-1’, and in Jowulu it is used only for ‘7’ (= 10-3), ‘8’ (=10-2), ‘9’ (= 10-1). In Bozo and San-Maka the subtractive numeral is ‘70’ (= 80-10), and in Dzuungoo it is ‘140’ (= 80x2 – 20); these cases can be understood in the context of the octogesimal systems, where the fundamental base is 80. In Dzuungoo, most probably, a subtractive numeral ‘70 = 80-10’ also existed at an earlier stage, but it has been ousted by a non-subtractive form.

212Although subtraction is not particularly rare in Mande counting systems, the internal syntax of subtractive numerals is idiosyncratic for every language, and as such is hardly reconstructible at the proto-level.

213Division is found in three numeral systems: Old Bamana, Jogo-Ton, Jogo-Ligbi. All these cases confirm Greenberg's (1990: 280) observations that the denominator of the fraction is always 2 (or a power of 2), and also, that division is usually found in vigesimal systems.

214The coexistence of systems with different bases and of various arithmetic operations in one language makes possible different designations for one and the same number. So, in the archaic Bamana system, 70 can be expressed as màninkɛ̀mɛ ní tán (60+10), as kɛ̀mɛ tán ɲà (80–10) or as kɛ̀mɛ tán ká jɛ̀ (80–10). In Dzuungoo, 1000 can be designated by the compound numeral gúrú-sɔ̄n kó cànfóōdzyéè (800x1 + ((80x2) + 40)) or by the Jula loanword wāākērēn.

4.6. Inner syntax of complex numerals. Connectors

215The order of components in complex numerals formed by means of multiplication and addition is the same in all Mande languages: Base x a + b, where a is a multiplier, and b is the addend.

216Multiplication is expressed paratactically, with the solitary exception of the Yaure language where we find a multiplication connector pá (”10 pá N” for the numerals from 30 to 90 divisible by 10).

217As for addition, it can be expressed by various means in Mande languages. Four models predominate statistically:

  • simple juxtaposition, without any morphological marking:33 Vai, Looma,34 Kpelle, Dzuungoo, Bobo, Gban, Yaure, Mwan, Boko. This is a rather infrequent model for addition, and it is never the only model used for addition in a language. Such a marginal position of additive parataxis can be explained by its predominance in multiplication;
  • a coordinative-comitative construction:35 Modern and Old Bamana, Mandinka, Kakabe, Jogo, Ligbi, Jalkunan, Bozo-Tigemaxo, Soninke, Dzuungoo, Seenku, Jowulu, Beng, Mwan, Dan-Gwɛɛtaa, Tura, San-Maka, Boko. This model predominates in the languages of the savanna zone, while it is rarer and in fact rather marginal in the forest zone (its expansion in Tura might be explained by Manding influence);
  • superessive postpositions or extended expressions with such postpositions (in some of the languages, the postposition is fused): Jalkunan (?), Looma, Loko, Gban, Guro, Yaure, Mwan, Dan-Gwɛɛtaa, Kla-Dan, Mano, Goo, Wan. The use of superessive adpositions as connectors for numerals (also attested in other languages of the world, although not very frequently) is very well represented in Southern Mande languages and in Southwestern Mande, but practically absent in the rest of the family;
  • a noun ‘bone, unit’ (which is also used as a numerative), sometimes ‘3sg/refl + grain’ (‘its grain’): Kpelle, Gban, Guro, Yaure, Mwan, Dan-Gwɛɛtaa, Kla-Dan, Mano, Goo, Tura. A connector kai going back to the noun for ‘bone, grain’ appears in Liberian dialects of Looma (Vydrin 1987: 46). In most languages, this connector appears only before the terms for units, but in Kpelle it precedes both units and tens, and in Mwan the connector ɓɛ̄ (presumably from ɓɛ̄ ‘grain’) is preceded by mīā ‘20’, while vū ‘10’ is linked to the units by another connector, tā (a superessive postposition). As we have demonstrated, the connector ‘bone, grain, unit’ is very well attested in the Southern Mande group, and its existence in Kpelle and Liberian Looma may result from Southern Mande influence.

218There are also some models attested only in single languages:

  • the nominal expression ‘its child’ (Bobo);
  • the expression ‘behind it’ (Vai);
  • the reciprocal pronoun (Wan);
  • an article-like suffix (Jalkunan);
  • the verb ‘add’ (Jowulu);
  • the possessive marker (San-Maka; however, possessive markers in Mande languages often go back to locative postpositions).

219Some connectors are of unknown origin: àsḭ́ŋ́ in Beng; wɛ́ɛ̏ in Seenku, a specialized postposition used only in compound numerals; lù in San-Maka.

220In certain languages, only one additive model is used in all cases. These are: Old and Modern Bamana, Mandinka, Kakabe, Jogo, Ligbi, Susu, Bozo-Tigemaxo, Soninke. In this group we find languages of large communities which have been involved in long-range trade for many centuries. In languages of the forest zone, we find multiple models for addition. Presumably, the presence of such “less standardized” systems correlates with a lower level of involvement in commercial relations and, therefore, less frequent use of higher numerals in the daily life of the communities.

4.7. External morphosyntax of numerals

4.7.1. Word order.

221The basic word order in the numerative construction in Mande is “Noun + Numeral”. A notable exception from this rule is represented by Soninke where numerals from 20 precede the noun, and in compound numerals, where elements for higher orders (from 20 on) precede the noun, while the elements for smaller numbers follow it (see 2.7).

222At the same time, certain elements resembling the Soninke model can also be found in other languages of the family. In particular, in compound numerals in Guinean Kpelle (Maria Konoshenko, p.c.), the noun can be optionally repeated between the orders of thousands and hundreds (17), although, unlike in Soninke, the noun obligatorily appears to the left of the entire compound numeral.

(17) pɛ́lɛ́ wáá hwéélɛ̀ (pɛ́lɛ́) ŋùŋ lɔ́ɔ́lí
house 1000 2 house 100 5
‘2500 houses’

223Repetition of the noun before each order of numerals is also the norm in Dan-Gwɛɛtaa (see 2.5), Goo, Guro and Seenku.

4.7.2. Plural markers.

224By default, in Mande numerative constructions, neither the noun, nor the numeral carries a plural marker. There are two exceptions in this respect: Soninke, where nouns appear in their plural forms before the numerals from 2 through 9, and Seenku, where nouns are always pluralized before numerals higher than 1.

225At the same time, in many languages the plural marker can be added to either the noun or the numeral, and in both cases the semantics of definiteness is expressed. This phenomenon is attested in Bamana (2.1), Mandinka (3.1) and Dzuungoo (2.3), and its existence cannot be excluded for some other languages (for which the necessary evidence may be missing at this time). In Dan-Gwɛɛtaa, a plural marker can be added to any element of the numerative construction, but the semantics of this is different from Bamana or Dzuungoo; it expresses restrictive meaning (”exactly the indicated number of items”). In Guro, a plural marker in a numerative construction expresses focus.

5. On the historical evolution of Mande counting systems

226One can make some suggestions concerning the sequence of emergence of different counting bases in Mande languages (which can probably be extended to other African languages as well).

  • The quinary system is based on counting on one's fingers: it is the most archaic model, which emerged before commerce appeared.
  • There are also traces of non-quinary models for the numbers of the first ten (in particular, a probable instance of te “Neo-2” strategy in Beng, San-Maka, and especially in Wan). These sporadic non-quinary elements seem to be quite archaic.
  • The presumable etymological links of the numerals for ‘100’ with ‘mouth’ in Gban and with ‘head’ in Mano, Kpelle and Looma are indicative of the existence, at an earlier stage, of extended body-tallying count systems similar to those described for Haruai by Comrie (1999).
  • The decimal model is an extension of the quinary system. In languages of large communities with a long history of state-building and involvement in long-distance trade, quinary systems tend to become blurred or disappear altogether.
  • The pentadecimal and vigesimal models can be considered as further extensions of “finger count systems”; more precisely, they can be defined as “finger and toe count systems”. In our sample, they are represented in 12 languages (out of 30). In many Mande languages, elements of vigesimal systems are rudimentary: there is a special word for ‘20’ (often going back to a lexeme for ‘human being’), while other numerals divisible by 20 belong to the decimal system (40 = 4x10, 60 = 6x10, etc.). Those languages which have more substantial (”extended”) vigesimal systems may follow two different models: numerals divisible by 20 are formed through multiplication of the numeral ‘20’ (40 = 2x20, 60 = 3x20, etc., as in Guro, Mwan, Bobo, etc., a “true vigesimal system”) or they are represented by different stems (as in the old Bamana system, Dzuungoo, Bozo, a “heteroradical vigesimal system”). The languages with extended vigesimal systems belong to different Mande groups, a fact which makes problematic the reconstruction of such a model for Proto-Mande (and even for the proto-languages of the lower-level groups within Mande). Instead, the geographical distribution of these languages makes plausible the hypothesis that the vigesimal model was borrowed from Senufo and/or Gur languages (where this model is much more widespread); in South Mande languages, the vigesimal model can also be explained as the result of Kru influence (on the spread of vigesimal systems in Kru, see (Zepp 1983)).
    The elements of pentadecimal systems in Jogo-Ligbi and Boko, isolated within the Mande family, may also result from borrowing from Gur languages or Senufo (cf. (Carlson 1994) on the presence of the pentadecimal model in some Senufo languages).
  • The octogesimal models (old Bamana, Dzuungoo, San-Maka) stem from the commercial relations of the pre-colonial period: they reflect a count based on bunches of cowrie shells (the predominant pre-colonial currency) or packages of cola nuts (the main item of intra-African trade).
  • The consistent decimal systems are the most innovative: they continue to expand and in the process are ousting more archaic systems, cf. the replacement of the old Bamana system by the new one.

227The dynamics of the evolution of numerical systems is also reflected in the use of connectors. In languages spoken by communities involved in large-scale trade for many centuries, it is typical to have one universal connector (normally a coordinative/comitative conjunction/preposition), while languages spoken in the forest zone, not used until very recently for large-scale trade, tend to employ various connectors for different orders.

228 

Haut de page

Bibliographie

Anonym. 2003. Boo nεn sέwε san-fransi, fransi-san [Lexique san–français, français–san]. Ouagadougou: Société internationale de linguistique.

Anonyme. 1982. Guide de Transcription et Lexique Bozo - Tieyaxo sawananbaana yee a xarabuye. Bamako: DNAFLA.

Aplonova, Ekaterina & Valentin Vydrin. 2017. Goo jazyk (Гоо язык) [Goo]. In Valentin Vydrin, Yulia Mazurova, Andrej Kibrik & Elena Markus (eds.), Jazyki mira: Jazyki mande (Языки мира: Языки манде) [Languages of the world: Mande languages], 457–469. St. Petersburg: Nestor-Historia.

Berthelette, John. 2001. Survey report on the San (Samo) language. SIL International. https://www.sil.org/resources/publications/entry/8983 (27 November, 2017).

Blecke, Thomas. 1996. Lexikalische Kategorien und grammatische Strukturen im Tigemaxo (Bozo, Mande) (Mande languages and linguistics 1). Köln: Rüdiger Köppe Verlag.

Bris, Pierre le & André Prost. 1981. Dictionnaire bobo – français, précédé d’une introduction grammaticale et suivi d’un Lexique français – bobo. Paris: SELAF.

Carlson, Robert. 1993. A sketch of Jɔ: A Mande language with a feminine pronoun. Mandenkan 25. 1–109.

Carlson, Robert. 1994. A grammar of Supyire (Mouton Grammar Library 14). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

Comrie, Bernard. 1997. Typology of numeral systems. https://mpi-lingweb.shh.mpg.de/numeral/TypNumCuhk_11ho.doc.

Comrie, Bernard. 1999. Haruai numerals and their implications for the history and typology of numeral systems. In Jadranka Gvozdanović (ed.), 81–94. Mouton de Gruyter. Berlin, New York.

Comrie, Bernard. 2005. Numeral Bases. In Martin Haspelmath, Matthew S. Dryer, David Gil & Bernard Comrie (eds.), The world atlas of language structures, 530–533. Oxford: Oxford University Press. http://wals.info/feature/131 (27 November, 2017).

Craig, Barry. 2010. Tally systems of the Upper Sepik and Central New Guinea. https://static1.squarespace.com/static/576ed271bebafbef665249c0/t/58b3504346c3c45d74e822bb/1488146500258/17-02+TallySystems.pdf.

Creissels, Denis & Pierre Sambou. 2013. Le mandinka. Phonologie, grammaire, textes. Paris: Karthala.

Creissels, Denis & Anna Urmanchieva. 2017. Soninke jazyk (Сонинке язык) [Soninke]. In Valentin Vydrin, Yulia Mazurova, Andrej Kibrik & Elena Markus (eds.), Jazyki mira: Jazyki mande (Языки мира: Языки манде) [Languages of the world: Mande languages], 248–283. St. Petersburg: Nestor-Historia.

Curtin, Philip D. 1975. Economic change in precolonial Africa: Senegambia in the era of the slave trade. Vol. 1. Madison, Wisconsin: The University of Wisconsin Press.

Delafosse, Maurice. 1929. La langue mandingue et ses dialectes (malinké, bambara, dioula). Paris: Geuthner.

Delafosse, Maurice. 1955. La langue mandingue et ses dialectes (malinké, bambara, dioula). Vol. 2. Paris: Imprimerie Nationale – Paul Geuthner.

Diagana, Ousmane Moussa. 1995. La langue soninkée: morphosyntaxe et sens à travers le parler de Kaédi (Mauritanie). Paris: Harmattan.

Diagana, Yacouba. 1994. Éléments de grammaire du soninké. 2 vols. Paris: Les documents de Linguistique africaine.

Erman, Anna. 2005. Le grammaticalisateur -ga en dan-blo. Mandenkan 41. 41–61.

Fedotov, Maksim. 2017. Gban jazyk (Гбан язык) [Gban]. In Valentin Vydrin, Yulia Mazurova, Andrej Kibrik & Elena Markus (eds.), Jazyki mira: Jazyki mande (Языки мира: Языки манде) [Languages of the world: Mande languages], 902–999. St. Petersburg: Nestor-Historia.

Grandet, E. 1973. La numération cardinale dans quelques langues de Côte d’Ivoire. Annales de l’Université d’Abidjan, Sér. H (Linguistique) VI(1). 47–102.

Greenberg, Joseph H. 1978. Generalizations about Numeral Systems. In Joseph H. Greenberg, Charles A. Ferguson & Edith A. Moravcsik (eds.), Universlals of human language: Word structure, vol. 3, 249–295. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

Greenberg, Joseph H. 1990. Generalizations about Numeral Systems. In Keith Denning & Suzanne Kemmer (eds.), On language: Selected writings of Joseph H. Greenberg, 271–309. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

Hammarström, Harald. 2010. Rarities in numeral systems. In Jan Wohlgemuth & Michael Cysouw (eds.), Rethinking universals: How rarities affect linguistic theory, 11–59. De Gruyter Mouton. Berlin.

Hanke, Thomas. 2010. Additional rarities in the typology of numerals. In Jan Wohlgemuth & Michael Cysouw (eds.), Rethinking universals: How rarities affect linguistic theory, 62–89. De Gruyter Mouton. Berlin.

Haspelmath, Martin. 2009. Lexical borrowing: concepts and issues. In Martin Haspelmath & Uri Tadmor (eds.), Loanwords in the world’s language: A Comparative Handbook, 35–54. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.

Heath, Jefferey. 2017. A grammar of Jalkunan (Mande, Burkina Faso). https://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/handle/2027.42/139025.

Hurford, James. 1975. The linguistic theory of numerals (Cambridge Studies in Linguistics 16). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Hurford, James. 1987. Language and Number. The Emergence of a Cognitive System. Oxford University Press. Oxford/New-York.

Idiatov, Dmitry. 2005. The exceptional morphology of Tura numerals and restrictors: Endoclitics, infixes and pseudowords. Journal of African Languages and Linguistics 26(1). 31–78. doi:10.1515/jall.2005.26.1.31.

Idiatov, Dmitry & Ekaterina Aplonova. 2017. Tura jazyk (Тура язык) [Tura]. In Valentin Vydrin, Yulia Mazurova, Andrej Kibrik & Elena Markus (eds.), Jazyki mira: Jazyki mande (Языки мира: Языки манде) [Languages of the world: Mande languages], 583–616. St. Petersburg: Nestor-Historia.

Jones, Ross. 1998. The Boko/Busa language cluster. München: Lincom Europa.

Jones, Ross. 2004. Boko dictionary with reversed English-Boko finderlist. München: Lincom Europa.

Khachaturyan, Maria. 2017. Mano jazyk (Мано язык) [Mano]. In Valentin Vydrin, Yulia Mazurova, Andrej Kibrik & Elena Markus (eds.), Jazyki mira: Jazyki mande (Языки мира: Языки манде) [Languages of the world: Mande languages], 680–718. St. Petersburg: Nestor-Historia.

Konoshenko, Maria. 2017. Kpelle jazyk (Кпелле язык) [Kpelle]. In Valentin Vydrin, Yulia Mazurova, Andrej Kibrik & Elena Markus (eds.), Jazyki mira: Jazyki mande (Языки мира: Языки манде) [Languages of the world: Mande languages], 284–343. St. Petersburg: Nestor-Historia.

Konta, Mahamadou & Valentin Vydrin. 2014. Propositions pour l’orthographe du bamanankan. Mandenkan 52. 22–54. doi:10.4000/mandenkan.318.

Kushnir, Elizaveta. 2017. Jaure jazyk (Яурэ язык) [Yauré]. In Valentin Vydrin, Yulia Mazurova, Andrej Kibrik & Elena Markus (eds.), Jazyki mira: Jazyki mande (Языки мира: Языки манде) [Languages of the world: Mande languages], 877–901. St. Petersburg: Nestor-Historia.

Kuznetsova, Natalia & Olga Kuznetsova. 2017. Guro jazyk (Гуро язык) [Guro. In Valentin Vydrin, Yulia Mazurova, Andrej Kibrik & Elena Markus (eds.), Jazyki mira: Jazyki mande (Языки мира: Языки манде) [Languages of the world: Mande languages], 765–877. St. Petersburg: Nestor-Historia.

Kuznetsova, Olga. 2008. Chislitel’nyje v jazyke guro (Числительные в языке гуро) [Numerals in the Guro language]. In Valentin Vydrin (ed.), African Collection – 2007, 410–417. St. Petersburg: Nauka.

Lafage, Susanne. 2002. Le lexique français de Côte d’Ivoire (appropriation et créativité). Le français en Afrique. Revue du Réseau des Observatoires du Français Contemporain en Afrique 16–17. http://www.unice.fr/bcl/ofcaf/16/16.html.

Lagarde, Michel. 1988. Influences de l’arabe sur la langue bambara. Falajè: Centre d’étude de langue bambara de Falajè (Mali).

Makeeva, Nadezhda. 2017. Kla-dan jazyk (Кла-дан язык) [Kla-Dan]. In Valentin Vydrin, Yulia Mazurova, Andrej Kibrik & Elena Markus (eds.), Jazyki mira: Jazyki mande (Языки мира: Языки манде) [Languages of the world: Mande languages], 617–679. St. Petersburg: Nestor-Historia.

Mills, Richard. 2003. Dictionnaire Sénoufo-Français: Sénanri - parler thébara (Côte d’Ivoire), avec un index français-thébara (Gur Monographs / Monographies Voltaïques 5). 2 vols. Köln: Rüdiger Köppe Verlag.

Mishchenko, Daria. 2017. Looma jazyk (Лоома язык) [Looma]. In Valentin Vydrin, Yulia Mazurova, Andrej Kibrik & Elena Markus (eds.), Jazyki mira: Jazyki mande (Языки мира: Языки манде) [Languages of the world: Mande languages], 343–414. St. Petersburg: Nestor-Historia.

Molin, Paul-Marie. 1956. Grammaire bambara. Issy-les Moulineaux: Presses Missionnaires.

Nikitina, Tatiana. 2017. Wan jazyk (Уан язык) [Wan]. In Valentin Vydrin, Yulia Mazurova, Andrej Kibrik & Elena Markus (eds.), Jazyki mira: Jazyki mande (Языки мира: Языки манде) [Languages of the world: Mande languages], 1032–1050. St. Petersburg: Nestor-Historia.

Nikitina, Tatiana. 2018. Verb phrase external arguments in Mande: New evidence for obligatory extraposition. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory. doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s11049-018-9417-0.

Nikitina, Tatiana. Dictionnaire wan-français-anglais-russe.

Olderogge, Dmitry. 1982. Sistemy scheta v jazykakh narodov Tropicheskoj i Juzhnoj Afriki (Системы счета в языках народов Тропической и Южной Африки) [Counting systems in languages of Tropical and Southern Africa]. Afrikansky Etnograficheskij Sbornik, vol. XIII, 1–33. Leningrad: Nauka.

Olderogge, Dmitry. 1984. Sistemy scheta v jazykakh narodov Tropicheskoj i Juzhnoj Afriki. Dvadcatirichnyje sistemy scheta (Системы счета в языках народов Тропической и Южной Африки. Двадцатиричные системы счета) [Counting systems in languages of Tropical and Southern Africa: Vigesimal systems]. Afrikansky Etnograficheskij Sbornik, vol. XIV, 3–16. Leningrad: Nauka.

Paperno, Denis & Anna Maloletnyaya. 2017. Ben jazyk (Бен язык) [Beng]. In Valentin Vydrin, Yulia Mazurova, Andrej Kibrik & Elena Markus (eds.), Jazyki mira: Jazyki mande (Языки мира: Языки манде) [Languages of the world: Mande languages], 1000–1032. St. Petersburg: Nestor-Historia.

Paré, Moïse. 1999. Dérivation, composition et syntagmes nominaux en san (parler de Yaba). Ouagadougou: Université d’Ouagadougou Rapport de D.E.A.

Perekhvalskaya, Elena. 2017. Mwan jazyk (Муан язык) [Mwan]. In Valentin Vydrin, Yulia Mazurova, Andrej Kibrik & Elena Markus (eds.), Jazyki mira: Jazyki mande (Языки мира: Языки манде) [Languages of the world: Mande languages], 718–765. St. Petersburg: Nestor-Historia.

Perekhvalskaya, Elena V. 2012. Chislitel’nyje v juzhnykh mande (Числительные в южных манде) [Numerals in South Mande languages]. In Valentin Vydrin & Alexander Zheltov (eds.), Mezhdu Nigerom i Kongo: zametki na pol’akh. K 60-letiju Konstantina Igorevicha Pozdniakova (Между Нигером и Конго: Заметки на полях. К 60-летию Константина Игоревича Позднякова) [Between the Niger and the Congo: Field notes. To the 60th anniversary of Konstantin Pozdniakov], 161–169. St. Petersburg: Nestor-Historia.

Persson, Andrew & Janet Persson. 1976. Word list of Brawhani Ligbi (Numu).

Persson, Andrew & Janet Persson. 2007. Ligbi numerals.

Pozdniakov, Konstantin. 2018. The Numeral System of Proto-Niger-Congo: A step-by-step reconstruction (Niger-Congo Comparative Studies 2). Berlin: Language Science Press. http://langsci-press.org/catalog/book/191.

Prost, André. 1958. Quelques notes sur le don (samogho). Bulletin de l’Institut Fondamental d’Afrique Noire, Série B 3–4. 613–623.

Prost, André. 1971. Eléments de sembla: phonologie, grammaire, lexique (Haute Volta: groupe mandé) (Afrique et Langage, Documents 5). Lyon.

Sapozhnikova, Maria. 2017. Dzhogo jazyk (Джого язык) [Jogo]. In Valentin Vydrin, Yulia Mazurova, Andrej Kibrik & Elena Markus (eds.), Jazyki mira: Jazyki mande (Языки мира: Языки манде) [Languages of the world: Mande languages], 213–220. St. Petersburg: Nestor-Historia.

Sauvant, Mgr. 1942. Manuel bambara. 4th edn. Maison-Carrée (Alger): Mission d’Afrique des Pères Blancs.

Saxe, Geofferey & Indigo Esmonde. 2004. Making change in Oksapmin tradestores: a study of shifting practices of quanfication under conditions of rapid shift towards a cash economy. South Pacific Journal of Phychology 15(1). 11–28.

Schreiber, Henning. 2008. Eine historische Phonologie der Niger-Volta-Sprachen (Mande Languages and Linguistics / Langues et Linguistique Mandé 7). Köln: Rüdiger Köppe Verlag.

Shluinsky, Andrey. 2017. Susu jazyk (Сусу язык) [Susu]. In Valentin Vydrin, Yulia Mazurova, Andrej Kibrik & Elena Markus (eds.), Jazyki mira: Jazyki mande (Языки мира: Языки манде) [Languages of the world: Mande languages], 221–247. St. Petersburg: Nestor-Historia.

Solomiac, Paul. 2014. Phonologie et morphosyntaxe du dzùùngoo de Samogohiri (Mande Languages and Linguistics / Langues et Linguistique Mandé 10). Köln: Rüdiger Köppe Verlag.

Touré, Aboubacar. 1994. Eléments de description de la langue soso. Grenoble: Université Stendhal-Grenoble III Ph.D. dissertation.

Van den Avenne, Cécile. 2017. L’exploration coloniale en ses langues: Une philologie des carnets africains du capitaine Binger. Genèses 106(1). 131–153. doi:10.3917/gen.106.0131.

Vanhoudt, Bettie. 1999. Lexique bisa-français. Mandenkan 34. 1–113.

Vydrin, Valentin. 1987. Jazyk looma (Язык лоома) [Looma language] (Jazyki narodov Azii i Afriki). Moscow: Nauka: Glavnaja redakcija vostochnoj literatury.

Vydrin, Valentin. 2009. On the problem of the Proto-Mande homeland. Journal of language relationship 1. 107–142.

Vydrin, Valentin. 2010. Co-ordinative pronouns in Southern and South-Western Mande: A second compound pronouns area in Africa? In Konstantin Pozdniakov, Valentin Vydrin & Alexander Zheltov (eds.), Personal pronouns in Niger-Congo languages: International workshop. St. Petersburg, September 13-15, 2010. Abstracts and papers: Working materials, 164–172. St. Petersburg: St. Petersburg University Press.

Vydrin, Valentin. 2017. Dan jazyk (Дан язык) [Dan]. In Valentin Vydrin, Yulia Mazurova, Andrej Kibrik & Elena Markus (eds.), Jazyki mira: Jazyki mande (Языки мира: Языки манде) [Languages of the world: Mande languages], 469–583. St. Petersburg: Nestor-Historia.

Vydrin, Valentin & Maria Morozova. 2017. Loko jazyk (Локо язык) [Loko]. In Valentin Vydrin, Yulia Mazurova, Andrej Kibrik & Elena Markus (eds.), Jazyki mira: Jazyki mande (Языки мира: Языки манде) [Languages of the world: Mande languages], 414–456. St. Petersburg: Nestor-Historia.

Vydrin, Valentin & Elena Perekhvalskaya. 2015. Sistemy schislenija v jazykakh mande (Системы счисления в языках манде) [Counting systems in Mande languages]. In Alexander Zheltov (ed.), Afrikanskij sbornik - 2015 (Африканский сборник - 2015) [African Collection - 2015], 356–377. St. Petersburg: Muzej antropologii i etnografii RAN (Kunstkamera).

Vydrina, Alexandra. 2017. Kakabe jazyk (Какабе язык) [Kakabe]. In Valentin Vydrin, Yulia Mazurova, Andrej Kibrik & Elena Markus (eds.), Jazyki mira: Jazyki mande (Языки мира: Языки манде) [Languages of the world: Mande languages], 172–212. St. Petersburg: Nestor-Historia.

Vydrine, Valentin. 2007. Les adjectifs en dan-gwèètaa. Mandenkan 43. 77–103.

Welmers, William. 1976. A Grammar of Vai. Berkeley: University of California Press.

Zepp, Raymond. 1983. L’apprentissage du calcul dans les langues de Côte-d’Ivoire. Université d’Abidjan. Institut de linguistique appliquée. Abidjan.

Haut de page

Notes

1 This work is related to research strand 3 “Typology and dynamics of linguistic systems” of the Labex EFL (financed by the ANR/CGI).

An earlier version of this study was published in Russian (Vydrin & Perekhvalskaya 2015), and numeral systems of Southern Mande were analyzed in (Perekhvalskaya 2012). However, the present paper is by no means a simple translation of those earlier articles: it is four times larger in size than (Vydrin & Perekhvalskaya 2015), the number of languages addressed has grown considerably, the mode of analysis has been radically changed, and the conclusions have been completely rewritten.

The authors thank Coleman Donaldson and Steven Kaye for their careful reading and numerous corrections of the English of the manuscript.

2 Cf. (Comrie 2005): “some languages have restricted numeral systems, by which I mean more specifically a numeral system that does not effectively go above around 20”.

3 For concrete examples of such systems see (Craig 2010; Saxe & Esmonde 2004).

4 See (Greenberg 1990: 281) on the importance of distinguishing between the augend and addend, the multiplicand and multiplier in natural numerical systems.

5 An anonymous reviewer commented that in these contexts, “it is not obvious that the numeral denotes 5 times its usual value, rather the head noun denotes a value of 5 francs”. That is true if we take expressions like Bamana dɔ́rɔmɛ fìla ‘10 CFA francs’, lit. ‘two five-francs’; however, the word dɔ́rɔmɛ in Bamana (and its analogues in the other languages) is very often omitted, and in this case the numeral appears without any head noun and still denotes five times its usual value.

6 Gourde is the name of the national currency of Haiti since 1813. The word seems to go back to the Spanish gordo in pesos gordo ‘hard peso’. It cannot be excluded that the forms gɔ̀lí (Mwan, Wan), gɔ̄lɩ̄, gɔ̄ɩ̄ (Guro) ‘5 francs’, etc. go back to the name of this ancient French coin.

7 In modern Guinean Maninka, the word dàlasi means ‘one Guinean franc’. This semantic shift (”5 francs > 1 franc”) is due to the intricate history of the monetary system of this country. Before independence, dàlasi meant ‘5 CFA francs’ (as everywhere else in French West Africa). After independence (1958), when Guinea left the CFA franc zone and introduced the Guinean franc, dàlasi still meant ‘5 (Guinean) francs’. In 1972, the Guinean franc was replaced by a new currency, the sily. In Maninka, the term dàlasi was retained, but from then on it meant ‘1 sily’, rather than 5. When in 1986 the sily was replaced again by the Guinean franc, the word dàlasi was kept for ‘1 Guinean franc’.

8 Kalilou Tera, personal communication.

9 The existence of these forms is confirmed by less regular data from the field notes (1887-1889) of Louis-Gustave Binger (Van den Avenne 2017: 146).

10 Most probably, toko is a Senufo form in origin, cf. tòkoo ‘20’ in Tyebara Senufo (Mills 2003: 1197) and toko ‘20’ in Teen (Pozdniakov 2018: 210); otherwise, it may be a common areal term for ‘20’ in the pre-colonial West Africa retained in some modern languages. Togo is used today in Ivorian French slang (i.e., in Nouchi; we are thankful to Coleman Donaldson for drawing our attention to this Nouchi form) for 100 francs. In Lafarge's dictionary (2002), this term is explained as follows: “par référence aux premières pièces qui portaient la mention AOF-Togo-Institut d'émission”. It seems however more plausible that it goes back to the precolonial term (5fr x 20 = 100fr).

11 In this book, forms for numerals are often presented with variable tones, without any explanation of what conditions this variability. We have had to choose among the variants in a more or less random way.

12 Therefore, Boko violates Greenberg's 12th (1990: 279) universal: “A subtrahend is always a simple lexical expression”.

13 However, according to Ross Jones (p.c.), sómȁ is an archaic word; younger people use “30+5” for ‘35’.

14 For ‘80’ some speakers use an extended form, bāmāā náá cɛ̀ɛ̀n, lit. ‘Senufo's 80’, which corresponds exactly to the bámanankɛme ‘80’ of Old Bamana (the original meaning of the term Bámàna, Bámbara is ‘pagan, non-Muslim’. It is locally applied to various ethnic groups).

15 In fact, no phonological reconstruction for Proto-Mande (or even the majority of the lower-level branches) has been carried out so far, so any etymological remarks can only be preliminary in nature. It can be said however that a semi-regular (secondary) correspondence “Bozo g- : Manding w- before back rounded vowels” can be confirmed by the following comparative series: ‘cola nut’ Bozo-Tigemaxo goro : Bamana wòro; ‘to smoke (meat, fish)’ Bozo-Tigemaxo gusu : Bamana wúsu, and some others.

16 In an unpublished list of Dzuungoo numerals (courtesy of Paul Solomiac), the numeral for 400 is spelled as cɛ̀ɛ̀n nún. In (Solomiac 2014: 155) it is spelled as one word and with a single n: cɛ̀ɛ̀nún.

17 In fact, gblɯ̋ in Dan may be etymologically related to the root WULU ~ WULI ~ GULU attested in many Mande languages for ‘1000’. The historical development *gúlú > *gwlú > *gblű > *gblɯ̋ would be quite normal and typical for Dan.

18 It is true that in San-Maka, ‘3’, sɔ̄ɔ̄, may look like a better candidate, however, this similarity is likely superficial, since the model ‘5+2’ is very typical of Mande languages, while ‘3+2 = 7’ makes no sense.

19 Frequently used in compound numerals in an abridged form, bīɛ̄ ‘bundle’, in which case the subsequent coordinative conjunction ká is omitted.

20 It cannot be excluded altogether that kàbú ‘9’ is a subtractive numeral in origin, in which case the element –bu/-bi (in Bozo, -wi, -fi, -pi) could be traced back to the archaic Mande root *pu ‘10’. It can be suggested (rather speculatively, as it is hard to prove or disprove) that the final element –mú in the numeral ‘10’ tànmú goes back to the the same archaic stem *pu.

21 Only the main features of the morphosyntax of Soninke numerals are analyzed, some peripheral ones have been left aside.

22 For example, in Mano ‘12’ is vṵ̀ dōó wɛ́lɛ́ pèèlɛ̄ (10 1 grain 2). Therefore, vṵ̀ cannot be used as an augend without being accompanied by a numeral for units (the multiplier), and it is therefore not mentioned in the column “Augends”.

23 We take as the standard test the ability or inability of a lexeme to express its basic meaning without being accompanied by the numeral ‘1’. So, in modern Bamana one may say mìsi kɛ̀mɛ ‘100 cows’, but to say ‘1000 cows’ the numeral kélen ‘1’ must be added (mìsi bà kélen). Therefore, kɛ̀mɛ ‘100’ is a numeral and bà ‘1000’ is a noun.
Another test is the possibility of forming an ordinal numeral on the basis of the lexeme in question. So, in modern Bamana, one can say mɔ̀gɔ kɛ̀mɛ-nan <human 100-ORD> ‘100th person’, but for 1000 no ordinal is directly available, only mɔ̀gɔ bà kélen-nan <human 1000 one-ORD> ‘1000th person’. However, ordinal numerals lie beyond the scope of this paper, and we cannot apply this test systematically (in fact, for many languages in our sample this information is not available); therefore, it was not taken into account.

24 Le Bris et Prost (1981: 39) mention that the root zɔ̀lō, pl. zɔ̀ō is “proper to South Mande languages” (in fact, they mean Eastern Mande), and they compare it with Bisa zol / zo ‘100’. However, Bisa seems to be the only South-Eastern Mande language where this root is attested.

25 The vowel i in the first syllable of the numeral ‘9’ can be explained by a regressive assimilation, *sɔ́-síé > sīsí.

26 The formal similarity of the words for ‘head’ in Mano, a South Mande language, and Kpelle-Looma, South-Western Mande languages, is not fortuitous and is not due to borrowing: in both groups, the forms for ‘head’ go back to the same Proto-Mande stem. However, the etymological link between ŋwū ‘100’ and ŋwū ‘head’ seems not to be perceived by speakers of Mano (Maria Khachaturyan, p.c.).

27 Related forms are attested in Dan-Blo (a western Ivoirian dialect of Dan) where ‘10’ in compound numerals is expressed by a form gɔ́ɔ̏ whose etymology is not clear, although a link with the word gɔ᷆ ‘cola’ cannot be excluded.

28 The “upper ring” symbol is used by Tatiana Nikitina for the “neutral high” tone, an underlying toneme whose superficial realization (high, mid or low) depends on contextual rules, see (Nikitina 2017: 1036; Nikitina 2018).

29 The areal character of this phenomenon is mentioned, in particular, in (Hammarström 2010: 32).

30 The element -rO- in Manding (and in Kakabe) probably goes back to the locative postposition *dɔ́ (as a counterpoint, however, it should be mentioned that this postposition has inessive meaning, while everywhere else connectors go back to superessive postpositions). On the other hand, it cannot be altogether excluded that the stem *GO- (or *GORO ?) could be related to the stem of the type KORO attested in Soninke-Bozo and Bobo; in that case, the element -rO- can be regarded as an integral part of the root. In this case, *GORO is a good candidate for reconstruction at the Proto-Western Mande level, in addition to *ɗọ́ọɗú. See also the discussion in 2.1.

31 The Jowulu couple ‘10’ byḭ̏ bȕ is unclear in this respect. At the present state of knowledge about this language, it is difficult to say whether these forms are derived from the same root or not.

32 For certain languages of the sample, this information is unavailable or irrelevant; these languages are not taken into account.

33 Languages where only addition in numerals for 6-9 is zero-marked are not taken into account.

34 Data for Loko are missing. It is highly probable that a zero connector is used in Loko in the same way as in Looma.

35 It is typical of Mande languages to express coordinative and comitative meanings by one and the same means. The marker of this construction may be a conjunction/preposition, or, in some languages, coordinative (inclusory) pronoun (Vydrin 2010).

Haut de page

Pour citer cet article

Référence électronique

Elena Perekhvalskaya et Valentin Vydrin, « Numeral systems in Mande languages »Mandenkan [En ligne], 61 | 2019, mis en ligne le 30 janvier 2020, consulté le 06 octobre 2024. URL : http://journals.openedition.org/mandenkan/1951 ; DOI : https://doi.org/10.4000/mandenkan.1951

Haut de page

Auteurs

Elena Perekhvalskaya

Institute for Linguistic Studies, St. Petersburg, Russia

Articles du même auteur

Valentin Vydrin

INALCO — LLACAN — St. Petersburg State University — IUF (Insitut universitaire de France)

Articles du même auteur

Haut de page

Droits d’auteur

CC-BY-NC-SA-4.0

Le texte seul est utilisable sous licence CC BY-NC-SA 4.0. Les autres éléments (illustrations, fichiers annexes importés) sont « Tous droits réservés », sauf mention contraire.

Haut de page
Rechercher dans OpenEdition Search

Vous allez être redirigé vers OpenEdition Search